Prestimulus EEG Power Predicts Conscious Awareness But Not Objective Visual Performance

Abstract Prestimulus oscillatory neural activity has been linked to perceptual outcomes during performance of psychophysical detection and discrimination tasks. Specifically, the power and phase of low frequency oscillations have been found to predict whether an upcoming weak visual target will be detected or not. However, the mechanisms by which baseline oscillatory activity influences perception remain unclear. Recent studies suggest that the frequently reported negative relationship between α power and stimulus detection may be explained by changes in detection criterion (i.e., increased target present responses regardless of whether the target was present/absent) driven by the state of neural excitability, rather than changes in visual sensitivity (i.e., more veridical percepts). Here, we recorded EEG while human participants performed a luminance discrimination task on perithreshold stimuli in combination with single-trial ratings of perceptual awareness. Our aim was to investigate whether the power and/or phase of prestimulus oscillatory activity predict discrimination accuracy and/or perceptual awareness on a trial-by-trial basis. Prestimulus power (3–28 Hz) was inversely related to perceptual awareness ratings (i.e., higher ratings in states of low prestimulus power/high excitability) but did not predict discrimination accuracy. In contrast, prestimulus oscillatory phase did not predict awareness ratings or accuracy in any frequency band. These results provide evidence that prestimulus α power influences the level of subjective awareness of threshold visual stimuli but does not influence visual sensitivity when a decision has to be made regarding stimulus features. Hence, we find a clear dissociation between the influence of ongoing neural activity on conscious awareness and objective performance.

[1]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Attention and Temporal Expectations Modulate Power, Not Phase, of Ongoing Alpha Oscillations , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  S. Saitoh,et al.  Epigenetic Inactivation and Subsequent Heterochromatinization of a Centromere Stabilize Dicentric Chromosomes , 2012, Current Biology.

[3]  C. Miniussi,et al.  The Functional Importance of Rhythmic Activity in the Brain , 2012, Current Biology.

[4]  H.A. Slagter,et al.  Facilitation and inhibition in attention: Functional dissociation of pre-stimulus alpha activity, P1, and N1 components , 2016, NeuroImage.

[5]  Lee M. Miller,et al.  The Role of Alpha Activity in Spatial and Feature-Based Attention , 2016, eNeuro.

[6]  Ryota Kanai,et al.  Rhythmic Influence of Top–Down Perceptual Priors in the Phase of Prestimulus Occipital Alpha Oscillations , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  Mingzhou Ding,et al.  Attentional Modulation of Alpha Oscillations in Macaque Inferotemporal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Frontoparietal Cortex Controls Spatial Attention through Modulation of Anticipatory Alpha Rhythms , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  R. Romo,et al.  α-Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  P. Corballis,et al.  Prestimulus alpha power influences response criterion in a detection task. , 2016, Psychophysiology.

[11]  Michael Zehetleitner,et al.  Visibility Is Not Equivalent to Confidence in a Low Contrast Orientation Discrimination Task , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[12]  W. El-Deredy,et al.  Pre-stimulus alpha oscillations over somatosensory cortex predict tactile misperceptions , 2017, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  O. Jensen,et al.  Shaping Functional Architecture by Oscillatory Alpha Activity: Gating by Inhibition , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  P. Rossini,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus alpha rhythms are related to conscious visual perception: a high-resolution EEG study. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  Fiona E. N. LeBeau,et al.  New horizons in the neuroscience of consciousness , 2010 .

[17]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The Role of Alpha-Band Brain Oscillations as a Sensory Suppression Mechanism during Selective Attention , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[18]  Maximilien Chaumon,et al.  Prestimulus Neural Oscillations Inhibit Visual Perception via Modulation of Response Gain , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Kyle E. Mathewson,et al.  Power and Phase of Alpha Oscillations Reveal an Interaction between Spatial and Temporal Visual Attention , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  G. Thut,et al.  Stimulus- and state-dependence of systematic bias in spatial attention: Additive effects of stimulus-size and time-on-task , 2012, Cortex.

[21]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  T. Ergenoğlu,et al.  Alpha rhythm of the EEG modulates visual detection performance in humans. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[23]  Nathaniel D. Daw,et al.  Self-Evaluation of Decision-Making: A General Bayesian Framework for Metacognitive Computation , 2017, Psychological review.

[24]  Viktor K. Jirsa,et al.  Relating Alpha Power and Phase to Population Firing and Hemodynamic Activity Using a Thalamo-cortical Neural Mass Model , 2015, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[25]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Dissociated α-band modulations in the dorsal and ventral visual pathways in visuospatial attention and perception. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  Hakwan Lau,et al.  The signal processing architecture underlying subjective reports of sensory awareness , 2016, Neuroscience of consciousness.

[27]  David Soto,et al.  On the independence of visual awareness and metacognition: a signal detection theoretic analysis. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  G. Thut,et al.  Mechanisms of selective inhibition in visual spatial attention are indexed by α‐band EEG synchronization , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  Hakwan Lau,et al.  Heuristic use of perceptual evidence leads to dissociation between performance and metacognitive sensitivity , 2016, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[30]  Caspar M. Schwiedrzik,et al.  Expecting to See a Letter: Alpha Oscillations as Carriers of Top-Down Sensory Predictions. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Prestimulus Oscillatory Activity in the Alpha Band Predicts Visual Discrimination Ability , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  C. Schroeder,et al.  Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[33]  C. Tagliabue,et al.  Early Local Activity in Temporal Areas Reflects Graded Content of Visual Perception , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[34]  N. Busch,et al.  Moment-to-moment fluctuations in neuronal excitability bias subjective perception rather than decision-making , 2017, bioRxiv.

[35]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  An Information-Maximization Approach to Blind Separation and Blind Deconvolution , 1995, Neural Computation.

[36]  Mingzhou Ding,et al.  From Prestimulus Alpha Oscillation to Visual-evoked Response: An Inverted-U Function and Its Attentional Modulation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  Ariel Zylberberg,et al.  The construction of confidence in a perceptual decision , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[38]  Michael Zehetleitner,et al.  Being confident without seeing: What subjective measures of visual consciousness are about , 2013, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[39]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[40]  R. VanRullen,et al.  An oscillatory mechanism for prioritizing salient unattended stimuli , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[42]  Sébastien M. Crouzet,et al.  Spontaneous Neural Oscillations Bias Perception by Modulating Baseline Excitability , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  R. Passingham,et al.  Relative blindsight in normal observers and the neural correlate of visual consciousness , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Measuring consciousness: Is one measure better than the other? , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[45]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  THE CONTROL OF THE FALSE DISCOVERY RATE IN MULTIPLE TESTING UNDER DEPENDENCY , 2001 .

[46]  M. Kendall Statistical Methods for Research Workers , 1937, Nature.

[47]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Reduced Occipital Alpha Power Indexes Enhanced Excitability Rather than Improved Visual Perception , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  J. Gross,et al.  Trial‐by‐trial co‐variation of pre‐stimulus EEG alpha power and visuospatial bias reflects a mixture of stochastic and deterministic effects , 2017, The European journal of neuroscience.

[49]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Spontaneous fluctuations in posterior alpha-band EEG activity reflect variability in excitability of human visual areas. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  H. Lau,et al.  Prestimulus Oscillatory Activity over Motor Cortex Reflects Perceptual Expectations , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  E. John,et al.  Perceptual framing and cortical alpha rhythm , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  R. VanRullen,et al.  At What Latency Does the Phase of Brain Oscillations Influence Perception? , 2017, eNeuro.

[53]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Increases in alpha oscillatory power reflect an active retinotopic mechanism for distracter suppression during sustained visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  A. Villringer,et al.  How Ongoing Neuronal Oscillations Account for Evoked fMRI Variability , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[56]  B. Postle,et al.  Prestimulus alpha-band power biases visual discrimination confidence, but not accuracy , 2016, Consciousness and Cognition.

[57]  Pieter R. Roelfsema,et al.  Variance misperception explains illusions of confidence in simple perceptual decisions , 2014, Consciousness and Cognition.

[58]  Diane M. Beck,et al.  Pulsed Out of Awareness: EEG Alpha Oscillations Represent a Pulsed-Inhibition of Ongoing Cortical Processing , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[59]  M. Overgaard,et al.  Introspection and subliminal perception , 2004 .

[60]  Niko A Busch,et al.  Moment-to-Moment Fluctuations in Neuronal Excitability Bias Subjective Perception Rather than Strategic Decision-Making , 2018, eNeuro.

[61]  R. VanRullen,et al.  The Phase of Ongoing EEG Oscillations Predicts Visual Perception , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  O. Andreassen,et al.  Mice Deficient in Cellular Glutathione Peroxidase Show Increased Vulnerability to Malonate, 3-Nitropropionic Acid, and 1-Methyl-4-Phenyl-1,2,5,6-Tetrahydropyridine , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  K. Linkenkaer-Hansen,et al.  Prestimulus Oscillations Enhance Psychophysical Performance in Humans , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  G. V. Simpson,et al.  Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific α-Bank Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[65]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[66]  Iain D. Gilchrist,et al.  The contribution of pre-stimulus neural oscillatory activity to spontaneous response time variability , 2015, NeuroImage.

[67]  R. VanRullen How to Evaluate Phase Differences between Trial Groups in Ongoing Electrophysiological Signals , 2016, bioRxiv.

[68]  Anders Lansner,et al.  Effect of Prestimulus Alpha Power, Phase, and Synchronization on Stimulus Detection Rates in a Biophysical Attractor Network Model , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  Ole Jensen,et al.  Frontal Eye Fields Control Attentional Modulation of Alpha and Gamma Oscillations in Contralateral Occipitoparietal Cortex , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[70]  C. Koch,et al.  Is perception discrete or continuous? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[71]  H. Lau,et al.  Prestimulus hemodynamic activity in dorsal attention network is negatively associated with decision confidence in visual perception. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[72]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  A shift of visual spatial attention is selectively associated with human EEG alpha activity , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[73]  Christopher W. Pleydell-Pearce,et al.  The phase of pre-stimulus alpha oscillations influences the visual perception of stimulus timing , 2016, NeuroImage.

[74]  Michael J. Morais,et al.  Global network influences on local functional connectivity , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[75]  R. VanRullen,et al.  Ongoing EEG Phase as a Trial-by-Trial Predictor of Perceptual and Attentional Variability , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[76]  B. Postle,et al.  Top-down control of the phase of alpha-band oscillations as a mechanism for temporal prediction , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[77]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.