Fear appeals and persuasion: the differentiation of a motivational construct.

THE goal of the present paper is to invite new thoughts about the effects of fear-arousing communications on persuasion. As a consequence, it is unashamedly theoretical and may seem to run counter to the insistent cries for the practical and relevant. But it is my thought that the theories and assumptions we make about other people are an important determinant of what we say when we try to influence or educate them. To do a better job in this role, we must be sure that we are using the most adequate theory in developing our persuasive and educational appeals. The best way to increase the quality of our theories is to develop and compare different ways of thinking about the same problem. Thus, to convince you that nothing is so practical as a good theory, I shall review two sets of ideas that can be used to understand the persuasive effects of fear-arousing health messages, discuss how well these ideas fit empirical data, and then leave you to decide which provides the most creative suggestions for practical action. The health professional's unique knowledge gives him the special responsibility to inform others of the imminence of health dangers. Because he wants his audience to believe and to act upon his recommendations, he is very likely to wonder about the most effective way of presenting his information. At this point the practitioner has to consider his options. Should he "hit them hard" and arouse strong fear with graphic descriptions of death and danger, or should he use a mild, unfrightening approach and avoid any mention of the serious hazards that may occur if one fails to execute a protective action? Which technique is most effective? If he has read the early study by Janis and Feshbach (1953) he would find support for a mild approach. This study suggests that increasing the vividness of the threat information on dental disease and thereby raising the level of fear decreases the proportion of subjects who follow a program of dental care. If he reads further, he is likely to notice that nearly all subsequent studies show that communications which stimulate higher levels of fear [on issues such as tetanus (Leventhal, Singer and Jones, 1965; Dabbs and Leventhal, 1966), smoking and lung cancer (Janis and Mann, 1965; Leventhal, Watts and Pagano, 1967)] lead to more acceptance of the communicator's recommendations.2 Indeed, he may notice that the number of studies with positive effects, high-fear messages producing more attitude and behavior change than low-fear messages,

[1]  L. Mann The effects of emotional role playing on desire to modify smoking habits , 1967 .

[2]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Persistence of Influence for Varying Durations of Exposure to Threat Stimuli , 1965, Psychological reports.

[3]  L. Berkowitz,et al.  The interest value and relevance of fear-arousing communications. , 1960, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[4]  Stanley Schachter,et al.  Cognitive manipulation of pain , 1966 .

[5]  G. Davison,et al.  Maintenance of self-attributed and drug-attributed behavior change. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Sex differences in attitude and behavior change under conditions of fear and specific instructions , 1966 .

[7]  D. Bem Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. , 1967, Psychological review.

[8]  J. C. Watts,et al.  Effects of fear and instructions on how to cope with danger. , 1967, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  H. Leventhal,et al.  EFFECTS OF FEAR AND SPECIFICITY OF RECOMMENDATION UPON ATTITUDES AND BEHAVIOR. , 1965, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Affect arousal and positioning of recommendations in persuasive communications. , 1966 .

[11]  Irving L. Janis,et al.  Effects of Fear Arousal on Attitude Change: Recent Developments in Theory and Experimental Research1 , 1967 .

[12]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Fear communications in the acceptance of preventive health practices. , 1965, Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine.

[13]  D. Cartwright,et al.  Some Principles of Mass Persuasion , 1949, Human relations; studies towards the integration of the social sciences.

[14]  I. Janis,et al.  A follow-up study on the long-term effects of emotional role playing. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  I. Janis,et al.  Effect of fear-arousing communications. , 1953, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[16]  I. Janis,et al.  Effectiveness of emotional role-playing in modifying smoking habits and attitudes. , 1965 .

[17]  S Epstein,et al.  Toward a unified theory of anxiety. , 1967, Progress in experimental personality research.

[18]  W. Weiss,et al.  EFFECTS OF THE MASS MEDIA OF COMMUNICATION. , 1966 .

[19]  James Michaels,et al.  Smoking: a behavioral analysis , 1971 .

[20]  G C Chu,et al.  Fear arousal, efficacy, and imminency. , 1966, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  R. Helmreich,et al.  Opinion change as a function of stress and communicator credibility , 1969 .

[22]  J. C. Watts,et al.  Sources of resistance to fear-arousing communications on smoking and lung cancer. , 1966, Journal of personality.

[23]  William Allen,et al.  Effects of fear-arousing communications , 1953 .

[24]  T. Brock,et al.  Behavioral receptivity to dissonant information. , 1967, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  J. Aronfreed The nature, variety, and social patterning of moral responses to transgression. , 1961, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[26]  B. Mausner,et al.  Individual Variation in Behavioral Change following Role Playing , 1969, Psychological reports.

[27]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Negative emotions and persuasion1 , 1968 .

[28]  D. Haefner,et al.  AROUSING FEAR IN DENTAL HEALTH EDUCATION , 1965 .

[29]  M A H Russell Smoking: A Behavioral Analysis , 1972 .

[30]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Findings and Theory in the Study of Fear Communications , 1970 .

[31]  R. Lazarus,et al.  Surprise versus suspense in the production of stress reaction. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.