Intact Motor Imagery in Chronic Upper Limb Hemiplegics: Evidence for Activity-Independent Action Representations

Four experiments were undertaken to examine the effects of chronic hemiplegia on the ability to internally represent actions involving either the paralyzed (contralesional) or relatively unaffected (ipsilesional) limb. An experimental group of chronic, densely hemiplegic patients was compared with controls who experienced nearly full recovery from an initially dense hemiparesis. All participants suffered cerebral vascular accidents that spared sites in premotor and parietal areas directly involved in representing upper limb actions. Despite chronic limb immobility, hemiplegic patients performed all four tasks at a high level of accuracy and showed no differences in their ability to represent actions of the contralesional versus ipsilesional limbs. On tasks that involved representing actions of the hands and lower arms, hemiplegic patients were as accurate as recovered controls. Hemiplegic patients were, however, less accurate than controls on a task that involved representing actions of either upper arm. Overall, chronic hemiplegics performed more accurately for decisions based on their contralesional limbs: a hemiplegic advantage that may be related to an ongoing focus on planning and/or imagining currently impossible movements. These findings reveal a dissociation between the ability to internally represent versus produce manual actions. Further, they demonstrate that internal action representations can be robust to even years of limb disuse.

[1]  S. Brunnstrom,et al.  Motor testing procedures in hemiplegia: based on sequential recovery stages. , 1966, Physical therapy.

[2]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[3]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental transformations in the identification of left and right hands. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  S. Gandevia The perception of motor commands or effort during muscular paralysis. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  L. Parsons Imagined spatial transformations of one's hands and feet , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  J. Kaas Plasticity of sensory and motor maps in adult mammals. , 1991, Annual review of neuroscience.

[8]  Susan J. Lederman,et al.  Cognitive representations of functional interactions with objects , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[9]  T. Weiss,et al.  Activation processes during mental practice in stroke patients. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[10]  L. Parsons Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  F Merten,et al.  Mental practice of motor skills used in poststroke rehabilitation has own effects on central nervous activation. , 1994, The International journal of neuroscience.

[12]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning , 1995, Nature.

[13]  M. Arbib,et al.  Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[14]  Y Agid,et al.  Congruent unilateral impairments for real and imagined hand movements , 1995, Neuroreport.

[15]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Mentally simulated movements in virtual reality: does Fitt's law hold in motor imagery? , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[16]  Driss Boussaoud,et al.  Frontal lobe mechanisms subserving vision-for-action versus vision-for-perception , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[17]  L. Parsons,et al.  Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET , 1995, Nature.

[18]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Use-dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional anatomy of pointing and grasping in humans. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.

[21]  R. Nudo,et al.  Neural Substrates for the Effects of Rehabilitative Training on Motor Recovery After Ischemic Infarct , 1996, Science.

[22]  R. Nudo,et al.  Reorganization of movement representations in primary motor cortex following focal ischemic infarcts in adult squirrel monkeys. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  Paul B. Johnson,et al.  Cortical networks for visual reaching: physiological and anatomical organization of frontal and parietal lobe arm regions. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  L Bozzao,et al.  Motor recovery after stroke. Morphological and functional brain alterations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  M W Marzke,et al.  Precision grips, hand morphology, and tools. , 1997, American journal of physical anthropology.

[26]  R. Nudo Remodeling of cortical motor representations after stroke: implications for recovery from brain damage , 1997, Molecular Psychiatry.

[27]  H. Branch Coslett,et al.  Evidence for a Disturbance of the Body Schema in Neglect , 1998, Brain and Cognition.

[28]  R. J. Seitz,et al.  A fronto‐parietal circuit for object manipulation in man: evidence from an fMRI‐study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of Primary Motor Cortex Affects Mental Rotation , 2022 .

[30]  S. H. Johnson,et al.  Thinking ahead: the case for motor imagery in prospective judgements of prehension , 2000, Cognition.

[31]  Scott H. Johnson,et al.  Imagining the impossible: intact motor representations in hemiplegics , 2000, Neuroreport.

[32]  Michael S Gazzaniga,et al.  Within grasp but out of reach: evidence for a double dissociation between imagined hand and arm movements in the left cerebral hemisphere , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  M. Wiesendanger,et al.  Different Ipsilateral Representations for Distal and Proximal Movements in the Sensorimotor Cortex: Activation and Deactivation Patterns , 2001, NeuroImage.

[34]  Mental practice. , 2002, Physical therapy.

[35]  Motor recovery after stroke , 2003 .

[36]  J. Donoghue,et al.  Dynamic organization of primary motor cortex output to target muscles in adult rats I. Long-term patterns of reorganization following motor or mixed peripheral nerve lesions , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  R. Johansson,et al.  Development of human precision grip I: Basic coordination of force , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Cognitive spatial-motor processes , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  J. Donoghue,et al.  Dynamic organization of primary motor cortex output to target muscles in adult rats II. Rapid reorganization following motor nerve lesions , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  R. Johansson,et al.  Development of human precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.