Spatial frequency-specific effects on the attentional bias: Evidence for two attentional systems
暂无分享,去创建一个
Matthias Niemeier | Ada Le | Ada Le | M. Niemeier | Boge Stojanoski | Vaughan V. W. Singh | Boge Stojanoski | Vaughan W. A. Singh | Matthias Niemeier
[1] M. McCourt,et al. Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[2] H. Karnath,et al. Disturbed line bisection is associated with posterior brain lesions , 2006, Brain Research.
[3] Ivan Toni,et al. Task instructions influence the cognitive strategies involved in line bisection judgements: evidence from modulated neural mechanisms revealed by fMRI , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[4] J. Rovamo,et al. Cortical magnification factor predicts the photopic contrast sensitivity of peripheral vision , 1978, Nature.
[5] Mark Mennemeier,et al. Biases in Attentional Orientation and Magnitude Estimation Explain Crossover: Neglect is a Disorder of Both , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[6] S. Ferber,et al. How to Assess Spatial Neglect - Line Bisection or Cancellation Tasks? , 2001, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.
[7] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[8] M. Carrasco,et al. Attention Alters the Appearance of Spatial Frequency and Gap Size , 2005, Psychological science.
[9] K. Seki,et al. Effects of cueing on visuospatial processing in unilateral spatial neglect , 1995, Journal of Neurology.
[10] M. Harvey,et al. Effects of visible and invisible cueing procedures on perceptual judgments in young and elderly subjects , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[11] M. Nicholls,et al. Can Free-Viewing Perceptual Asymmetries be Explained by Scanning, Pre-Motor or Attentional Biases? , 2002, Cortex.
[12] K. M. Heilman,et al. Contributions of the left and right cerebral hemispheres to line bisection , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[13] S. Black,et al. Brain-behavior correlations in hemispatial neglect using CT and SPECT , 1998, Neurology.
[14] Matthias Niemeier,et al. Stimulus-Driven and Voluntary Saccades Are Coded in Different Coordinate Systems , 2003, Current Biology.
[15] Matthias Niemeier,et al. Task-dependent differences in the exploratory behaviour of patients with spatial neglect , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[16] Roberto Cubelli,et al. Selective spatial attention and length representation in normal subjects and in patients with unilateral spatial neglect , 1989, Brain and Cognition.
[17] Christopher Kennard,et al. Visual neglect associated with frontal lobe infarction , 1996, Journal of Neurology.
[18] C. Kennard,et al. The anatomy of visual neglect , 2003 .
[19] J. Mattingley,et al. Can task specific perceptual bias be distinguished from unilateral neglect? , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[20] Allison B Sekuler,et al. Spatial characteristics of center-surround antagonism in younger and older adults. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[21] Michael I. Posner,et al. Components of neglect from right-hemisphere damage: An analysis of line bisection , 1990, Neuropsychologia.
[22] M. McCourt,et al. Unilateral Visual Cueing and Asymmetric Line Geometry Share a Common Attentional Origin in the Modulation of Pseudoneglect , 2005, Cortex.
[23] K. Zilles,et al. The Neural Basis of Vertical and Horizontal Line Bisection Judgments: An fMRI Study of Normal Volunteers , 2001, NeuroImage.
[24] M. Moscovitch,et al. Hemispheric control of spatial attention , 1990, Brain and Cognition.
[25] A. Davies,et al. Putting attention on the line: Investigating the activation–orientation hypothesis of pseudoneglect , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[26] N. Akbar,et al. The perceptual consequences of the attentional bias: evidence for distractor removal , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[27] A. Milner,et al. An Investigation of Hemispatial Neglect Using the Landmark Task , 1995, Brain and Cognition.
[28] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[29] S. Ferber,et al. Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe , 2001, Nature.
[30] T. Schenkenberg,et al. Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment , 1980, Neurology.
[31] Matthias Niemeier,et al. Simulating and testing visual exploration in spatial neglect based on a new model for cortical coordinate transformation , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[32] M D'Esposito,et al. Assessment of neglect reveals dissociable behavioral but not neuroanatomical subtypes , 1996, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[33] L. Rueckert,et al. Pseudoneglect and the cross-over effect , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[34] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[35] Padraic Monaghan,et al. Hemispheric asymmetries in cognitive modeling: connectionist modeling of unilateral visual neglect. , 2004, Psychological review.
[36] Daniel C. Javitt,et al. Right hemisphere control of visuospatial attention: line-bisection judgments evaluated with high-density electrical mapping and source analysis☆ , 2003, NeuroImage.
[37] John C. Marshall,et al. Left Visuo-Spatial Neglect: A Meaningless Entity? , 1992, Cortex.
[38] Mark E. McCourt,et al. Visuospatial attention in line bisection: stimulusmodulation of pseudoneglect , 1999, Neuropsychologia.
[39] P. Bartolomeo,et al. White matter (dis)connections and gray matter (dys)functions in visual neglect: Gaining insights into the brain networks of spatial awareness , 2008, Cortex.
[40] A. Milner,et al. Distortion of size perception in visuospatial neglect , 1995, Current Biology.
[41] M. Niemeier,et al. Paradoxical cross-over due to attention to high or low spatial frequencies , 2008, Brain and Cognition.
[42] J. Binder,et al. Distinct syndromes of hemineglect. , 1992, Archives of neurology.
[43] John C. Marshall,et al. How Long is a Piece of String? A Study of Line Bisection in a Case of Visual Neglect , 1988, Cortex.
[44] M. Nicholls,et al. Free-viewing perceptual asymmetries for the judgement of brightness, numerosity and size , 1999, Neuropsychologia.
[45] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[46] M. Nicholls,et al. The greyscales task: a perceptual measure of attentional bias following unilateral hemispheric damage , 2004, Neuropsychologia.
[47] K. Luh. Line bisection and perceptual asymmetries in normal individuals: What you see is not what you get. , 1995 .
[48] M. Niemeier,et al. Influences of time and spatial frequency on the perceptual bias: Evidence for competition between hemispheres , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[49] D. Perani,et al. The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man , 1986, Neuropsychologia.
[50] J. Sergent. The cerebral balance of power: confrontation or cooperation? , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[51] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. The effect of cueing on unilateral neglect , 1983, Neuropsychologia.
[52] K. Heilman,et al. Mechanisms underlying hemispatial neglect , 1979, Annals of neurology.
[53] H. Karnath,et al. The subcortical anatomy of human spatial neglect: putamen, caudate nucleus and pulvinar. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[54] Hanna Damasio,et al. Neglect following damage to frontal lobe or basal ganglia , 1980, Neuropsychologia.