Memory for pantomimed actions versus actions with real objects
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[2] E Donchin,et al. Studies of Squeezing: Handedness, Responding Hand, Response Force, and Asymmetry of Readiness Potential , 1974, Science.
[3] John D. Bransford,et al. Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing , 1977 .
[4] Peter Burke,et al. Log-linear models , 1980 .
[5] W. P. Dixon,et al. BMPD statistical software manual , 1988 .
[6] D. O. Hebb,et al. The organization of behavior , 1988 .
[7] A. Damasio. Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition , 1989, Cognition.
[8] K M Heilman,et al. Three-dimensional computergraphic analysis of apraxia. Neural representations of learned movement. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[9] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[10] A. P. Georgopoulos,et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. , 1993, Science.
[11] L. Jakobson,et al. Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[12] N. Motomura,et al. A Case of Ideational Apraxia with Impairment of Object Use and Preservation of Object Pantomime , 1994, Cortex.
[13] James L. McClelland,et al. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.
[14] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[15] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[16] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.
[17] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Face encoding and recognition in the human brain. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[18] V. Jousmäki,et al. Involvement of Primary Motor Cortex in Motor Imagery: A Neuromagnetic Study , 1997, NeuroImage.
[19] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[20] C. Petten,et al. Who said what? An event-related potential investigation of source and item memory. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[21] Morris Moscovitch,et al. Networks of domain-specific and general regions involved in episodic memory for spatial location and object identity , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[22] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[23] David A. Westwood,et al. Pantomimed actions may be controlled by the ventral visual stream , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[24] A. Berthoz,et al. Mental representations of movements. Brain potentials associated with imagination of eye movements , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[25] B. Ska,et al. Selective impairment of transitive gestures: An unusual case of apraxia , 1999 .
[26] Ava J. Senkfor,et al. Memory for drawings in locations: spatial source memory and event-related potentials. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[27] E. Tulving,et al. Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during memory retrieval. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[28] R. Cabeza,et al. Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[29] R. Cubelli,et al. Cognition in Action: Testing a Model of Limb Apraxia , 2000, Brain and Cognition.
[30] I Law,et al. Categorization and category effects in normal object recognition A PET Study , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[31] S. Petersen,et al. Memory's echo: vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[32] J. Moll,et al. Functional MRI correlates of real and imagined tool-use pantomimes , 2000, Neurology.
[33] Alex Martin,et al. Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.
[34] J. Decety,et al. Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta‐analysis , 2001, Human brain mapping.
[35] Anthony Randal McIntosh,et al. Transperceptual Encoding and Retrieval Processes in Memory: A PET Study of Visual and Haptic Objects , 2001, NeuroImage.
[36] T. Fukutake,et al. Apraxia of Tool Use: An Autopsy Case of Biparietal Infarction , 2002, European Neurology.
[37] H. Müller-Gärtner,et al. Brain systems engaged in encoding and retrieval of word-pair associates independent of their imagery content or presentation modalities , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[38] Olaf B. Paulson,et al. When Action Turns into Words. Activation of Motor-Based Knowledge during Categorization of Manipulable Objects , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[39] Episodic action memory: Characterization of the time course and neural circuitry , 2002 .
[40] John E Desmond,et al. Evidence for cortical encoding specificity in episodic memory: memory-induced re-activation of picture processing areas , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[41] Marta Kutas,et al. Episodic Action Memory for Real Objects: An ERP Investigation With Perform, Watch, and Imagine Action Encoding Tasks Versus a Non-Action Encoding Task , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[42] A Bartolo,et al. Pantomimes are special gestures which rely on working memory , 2003, Brain and Cognition.
[43] M. Brett,et al. Actions Speak Louder Than Functions: The Importance of Manipulability and Action in Tool Representation , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[44] D. Westwood,et al. Selective Dysfunction of Tool-Use: A Failure to Integrate Somatosensation and Action , 2003, Neurocase.
[45] R. Iansek,et al. Movement-related potentials associated with movement preparation and motor imagery , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.
[46] Lee Ryan,et al. An fMRI study of episodic memory: retrieval of object, spatial, and temporal information. , 2004, Behavioral neuroscience.
[47] J Hermsdörfer,et al. The effect of tactile feedback on pantomime of tool use in apraxia , 2004, Neurology.
[48] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.
[49] L. Buxbaum,et al. On beyond mirror neurons: internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[50] Joachim Hermsdörfer,et al. Manual and hemispheric asymmetries in the execution of actual and pantomimed prehension , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[51] L. Buxbaum,et al. Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[52] Kouichi Sutani,et al. Reactivation of physical motor information in the memory of action events , 2006, Brain Research.
[53] Marta Kutas,et al. Enactment versus conceptual encoding: Equivalent item memory but different source memory , 2008, Cortex.