A comparison of a child's fundamental frequencies in structured elicited vocalizations versus unstructured natural vocalizations: a case study.

OBJECTIVES Building on the concept that task type may influence fundamental frequency (F(0)) values, the purpose of this case study was to investigate the difference in a child's F(0) during structured, elicited tasks and long-term, unstructured activities. It also explores the possibility that the distribution in children's F(0) may make the standard statistical measures of mean and standard deviation less than ideal metrics. METHODS A healthy male child (5 years, 7 months) was evaluated. The child completed four voice tasks used in a previous study of the influence of task type on F(0) values: (1) sustaining the vowel /a/ in isolation; (2) sustaining the vowel /a/ embedded in a word at the end of a phrase; (3) repeating a sentence; and (4) counting from 1 to 10. The child also wore a National Center for Voice and Speech voice dosimeter, a device that collects voice data over the course of an entire day, during all activities for 34 h over 4 days. RESULTS Throughout the structured vocal tasks within the clinical environment, the child's F(0), as measured by both the dosimeter and acoustic analysis of microphone data, was similar for all four tasks, with the counting task the most dissimilar. The mean F(0) (approximately 257 Hz) matched very closely to the average task results in the literature given for the child's age group. However, the child's mean fundamental frequency during the unstructured activities was significantly higher (approximately 376 Hz). Finally, the mode and median of the structured vocal tasks were 260 Hz and 259 Hz respectively (both near the mean), while the unstructured mode and median were 290 Hz and 355 Hz respectively. CONCLUSIONS The results of this study suggest that children may produce a notably different voice pattern during clinical observations compared to routine daily activities. In addition, the child's long-term F(0) distribution is not normal. If this distribution is consistent in long-term, unstructured natural vocalization patterns of children, statistical mean would not be a valid measure. Mode and median are suggested as two parameters which convey more accurate information about typical F(0) usage. Finally, future research avenues, including further exploration of how children may adapt their F(0) to various environments, conversation partners, and activity, are suggested.

[1]  R. Nuss,et al.  Pediatric vocal nodules: correlation with perceptual voice analysis. , 2005, International journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology.

[2]  J. Perheentupa,et al.  Fundamental voice frequence during normal and abnormal growth, and after androgen treatment. , 1978, Archives of disease in childhood.

[3]  E. Yairi,et al.  Fundamental frequency, jitter, and shimmer in preschoolers who stutter. , 1992, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[4]  S N Awan,et al.  Speaking fundamental frequency characteristics of white, African American, and Hispanic kindergartners. , 1996, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[5]  H. Wertzner,et al.  Analysis of fundamental frequency, jitter, shimmer and vocal intensity in children with phonological disorders , 2005, Brazilian journal of otorhinolaryngology.

[6]  Z. Benyo,et al.  Acoustic analysis of the infant cry: classical and new methods , 2004, The 26th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[7]  Lawrence R. Rabiner,et al.  Analysis of Fundamental Frequency Contours , 1969 .

[8]  M. Robb,et al.  Developmental trends in vocal fundamental frequency of young children. , 1985, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[9]  D. Ashmead,et al.  The acoustic bases for gender identification from children's voices. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  Haydée Fiszbein Wertzner,et al.  Análise da freqüência fundamental, jitter, shimmer e intensidade vocal em crianças com transtorno fonológico , 2005 .

[11]  J. Švec,et al.  Vocal dose measures: quantifying accumulated vibration exposure in vocal fold tissues. , 2003, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[12]  Susan E Baker,et al.  The effect of task type on fundamental frequency in children. , 2008, International journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology.

[13]  Raymond D. Kent,et al.  Anatomical and neuromuscular maturation of the speech mechanism: evidence from acoustic studies. , 1976, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[14]  J. Švec,et al.  Measurement of vocal doses in speech: experimental procedure and signal processing. , 2003, Logopedics, phoniatrics, vocology.

[15]  Xuejing Sun,et al.  Pitch determination and voice quality analysis using Subharmonic-to-Harmonic Ratio , 2002, 2002 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing.

[16]  M P Robb,et al.  Vocal fundamental frequency characteristics during the first two years of life. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  G. G. Stokes "J." , 1890, The New Yale Book of Quotations.

[18]  E. Okur,et al.  The prevalence of vocal fold nodules in school age children. , 2004, International journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology.

[19]  Elisabeth Scheiner,et al.  Acoustic analyses of developmental changes and emotional expression in the preverbal vocalizations of infants. , 2002, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[20]  M. Robb,et al.  Fundamental frequency onset and offset behavior: a comparative study of children and adults. , 2002, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[21]  Ingo R. Titze,et al.  The Calibration and Setup of the NCVS Dosimeter , 2004 .

[22]  T Murry,et al.  Acoustic attributes of preadolescent voices. , 1980, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  Ingo R Titze,et al.  Adaptation of a Pocket PC for use as a wearable voice dosimeter. , 2005, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[24]  M A Crary,et al.  Production of linguistic prosody by normal and speech-disordered children. , 1993, Journal of communication disorders.

[25]  Ingo R Titze,et al.  Voicing and silence periods in daily and weekly vocalizations of teachers. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[26]  P F Assmann,et al.  Time-varying spectral change in the vowels of children and adults. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  I. Titze,et al.  Objective Measurement of Vocal Fatigue in Classical Singers: A Vocal Dosimetry Pilot Study , 2006, Otolaryngology--head and neck surgery : official journal of American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery.

[28]  A. Hudson,et al.  Spontaneous speaking fundamental frequency of 6-year-old black children. , 1988, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[29]  D N Sorenson,et al.  A fundamental frequency investigation of children ages 6-10 years old. , 1989, Journal of communication disorders.

[30]  J. Windsor,et al.  Acoustic and perceptual correlates of stress in nonwords produced by children with suspected developmental apraxia of speech and children with phonological disorder. , 2003, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[31]  H. Rothgänger,et al.  Analysis of the sounds of the child in the first year of age and a comparison to the language. , 2003, Early human development.

[32]  B Weinberg,et al.  Speaking fundamental frequency characteristics of five- and six-year-old children with mongolism. , 1970, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[33]  I. Hirsh,et al.  Development of speech sounds in children. , 1969, Acta oto-laryngologica. Supplementum.

[34]  S Bennett,et al.  Speaker sex recognition of 5- and 6-year-old children's voices. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[35]  E. Nwokah,et al.  Vocal affect in three-year-olds: a quantitative acoustic analysis of child laughter. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[36]  G. Böhme,et al.  Voice profiles and standard voice profile of untrained children. , 1995, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[37]  Shrikanth S. Narayanan,et al.  Acoustics of children's speech: developmental changes of temporal and spectral parameters. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[38]  Marcio Nogueira de Souza,et al.  Longitudinal study of the fundamental frequency of hunger cries along the first 6 months of healthy babies. , 2007, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[39]  G. Fairbanks An acoustical comparison of vocal pitch in seven- and eight-year-old children. , 1950, Child development.