The temporal properties of the human short-wave photoreceptors and their associated pathways

Flicker modulation sensitivity measurements made on high intensity orange steady backgrounds indicate that signals from short-wavelength sensitive cones (S-cones) have access to two pathways. At low S-cone adaptation levels the frequency response falls quickly with increasing frequency, but at higher adaptation levels it extends to much higher frequencies. At these higher S-cone adaptation levels, the following procedures can selectively expose either a process sensitive to low frequencies or one more sensitive to higher frequencies: (1) at high flicker frequencies, the S-cone signal can be nulled by a long-wavelength sensitive cone (L-cone) signal of suitable amplitude and phase, but at low frequencies a residual flicker persists; the modulation sensitivity for the residual flicker is lowpass in shape with a rapid decline in sensitivity with increasing flicker frequency; (2) sensitivity to flicker in the presence of a 17 Hz S- or L-cone mask is also lowpass with a similarly steep loss of high frequency sensitivity; yet (3) sensitivity to flicker during transient stimulation of the S-cones at 0.5 Hz is comparatively wideband (and slightly bandpass) in shape. The S-cone signal produced by the high frequency process is almost as well-maintained towards high frequencies as M- and L-cone signals. Furthermore, it is capable of participating in flicker photometric nulls with M- and L-cone signals. At low frequencies, however, when the low frequency S-cone signal is also present, satisfactory nulls can not be found. From these and phenomenological considerations, we identify the low and high frequency S-cone processes as S-cone inputs to the chromatic and luminance pathways, respectively. The phase adjustments needed to optimize flicker photometric nulls reveal that the S-cone input to the luminance pathway is actually inverted, but this is demonstrable only at relatively low frequencies: at medium or high frequencies the S-cone influence can be synergistic with that of the other cone types because of a delay in the transmission of S-cone signals.

[1]  E. Zrenner,et al.  Characteristics of the blue sensitive cone mechanism in primate retinal ganglion cells , 1981, Vision Research.

[2]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Spectral sensitivity by constant CFF: effect of chromatic adaptation. , 1973, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[3]  D. Norren,et al.  Transient tritanopia at the level of the ERGb-wave , 1979, Vision Research.

[4]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 nm , 1975, Vision Research.

[5]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Equiluminance: spatial and temporal factors and the contribution of blue-sensitive cones. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[6]  J. J. Wisowaty Estimates for the temporal response characteristics of chromatic pathways. , 1981, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[7]  H. D. L. Dzn Research into the Dynamic Nature of the Human Fovea→Cortex Systems with Intermittent and Modulated Light. II. Phase Shift in Brightness and Delay in Color Perception , 1958 .

[8]  G S Brindley,et al.  The flicker fusion frequency of the blue‐sensitive mechanism of colour vision , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  P. Lennie Recent developments in the physiology of color vision , 1984, Trends in Neurosciences.

[10]  H DE LANGE DZN,et al.  Research into the dynamic nature of the human fovea-cortex systems with intermittent and modulated light. II. Phase shift in brithtness and delay in color perception. , 1958, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[11]  J. Mollon,et al.  Transient tritanopia of a second kind , 1987, Vision Research.

[12]  P. Schiller,et al.  Properties and tectal projections of monkey retinal ganglion cells. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  C. V. Truss Chromatic flicker fusion frequency as a function of chromaticity difference. , 1957, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[14]  J. Moreland Retinal topography and the blue-arcs phenomenon. , 1969, Vision research.

[15]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Threshold temporal integration of chromatic stimuli , 1984, Vision Research.

[16]  William H. Merigan,et al.  Spatio-temporal vision of macaques with severe loss of Pβ retinal ganglion cells , 1986, Vision Research.

[17]  J. Mollon,et al.  Saturation of a retinal cone mechanism , 1977, Nature.

[18]  F. M. D. Monasterio Properties of concentrically organized X and Y ganglion cells of macaque retina. , 1978 .

[19]  Trichur Raman Vidyasagar,et al.  The responses of cells in macaque lateral geniculate nucleus to sinusoidal gratings. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  R. Shapley,et al.  X and Y cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque monkeys. , 1982, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  A. Mariani Bipolar cells in monkey retina selective for the cones likely to be blue-sensitive , 1984, Nature.

[22]  P. Lennie,et al.  Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivities of neurones in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  H. Ives XII. Studies in the photometry of lights of different colours , 1912 .

[24]  D. H. Kelly Spatio-temporal frequency characteristics of color-vision mechanisms* , 1974 .

[25]  E. N. Pugh,et al.  Temporal integration of the π1/ π3 pathway in normal and dichromatic vision , 1984, Vision Research.

[26]  C. B. Gillman,et al.  Factors affecting luminance additivity at threshold among normal and color-blind subjects and elaborations of a trichromatic-opponent colors theory. , 1968, Vision research.

[27]  J. Mollon,et al.  A theory of theΠ1 andΠ3 color mechanisms of stiles , 1979, Vision Research.

[28]  P. Lennie,et al.  Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[29]  W. Stiles,et al.  Saturation of the Rod Mechanism of the Retina at High Levels of Stimulation , 1954 .

[30]  P. Lennie,et al.  Mechanisms of color vision. , 1988, Critical reviews in neurobiology.

[31]  D. Hubel,et al.  Spatial and chromatic interactions in the lateral geniculate body of the rhesus monkey. , 1966, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  J. Mollon,et al.  An anomaly in the response of the eye to light of short wavelengths. , 1977, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[33]  J. Krauskopf,et al.  Reaction time as a measure of the temporal response properties of individua colour mechanisms. , 1973, Vision research.

[34]  C. Stromeyer,et al.  Apparent saturation of blue-sensitive cones occurs at a color-opponent stage. , 1978, Science.

[35]  D. Baylor,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. , 1987, The Journal of physiology.

[36]  D. Baylor,et al.  Photoreceptor signals and vision. Proctor lecture. , 1987, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[37]  R. M. Boynton,et al.  Temporal modulation sensitivity of the blue mechanism: Measurements made without chromatic adaptation , 1980, Vision Research.

[38]  J D Mollon,et al.  The independence of the temporal integration properties of individual chromatic mechanisms in the human eye , 1971, The Journal of physiology.

[39]  J. Pederson,et al.  Acetazolamide effect on the inward permeability of the blood-retinal barrier to carboxyfluorescein. , 1987, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[40]  D. Macleod,et al.  Blue-sensitive cones do not contribute to luminance. , 1980, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[41]  D. Tolhurst,et al.  Trichromatic colour opponency in ganglion cells of the rhesus monkey retina. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[42]  B. B. Lee,et al.  The physiological basis of heterochromatic flicker photometry demonstrated in the ganglion cells of the macaque retina. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[43]  J. Mollon,et al.  A curiosity of light adaptation , 1980, Nature.

[44]  E. Yund,et al.  Responses of macaque lateral geniculate cells to luminance and color figures. , 1977, Sensory processes.

[45]  E. Zrenner,et al.  Color coding in primate retina , 1981, Vision Research.

[46]  R. Weale Mechanisms of Colour Vision , 1979 .

[47]  A. Adams,et al.  Short-wavelength-sensitive cones do not contribute to mesopic luminosity. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[48]  E. J. Augenstein,et al.  The dynamics of the Π1 colour mechanism: further evidence for two sites of adaptation , 1977, The Journal of physiology.

[49]  C. Stromeyer,et al.  Contribution of human short‐wave cones to luminance and motion detection. , 1989, The Journal of physiology.

[50]  P. Padmos,et al.  Human and macaque blue cones studied with electroretinography. , 1973, Vision research.

[51]  D. G. Green,et al.  Sinusoidal flicker characteristics of the color-sensitive mechanisms of the eye. , 1969, Vision research.