The P-chain: relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Michael S. C. Thomas,et al. Modeling language acquisition in atypical phenotypes. , 2003, Psychological review.
[2] T. Florian Jaeger,et al. Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density , 2010, Cognitive Psychology.
[3] Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al. How language production shapes language form and comprehension , 2012, Front. Psychol..
[4] J. Fodor,et al. Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis , 1988, Cognition.
[5] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. A Rose by Any Other Name: Long-Term Memory Structure and Sentence Processing , 1999 .
[6] J B Oetting,et al. Children with SLI use argument structure cues to learn verbs. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[7] Marius Janciauskas,et al. Language adaptation and learning: Getting explicit about implicit learning , 2012, Lang. Linguistics Compass.
[8] Letitia R. Naigles,et al. Children use syntax to learn verb meanings , 1990, Journal of Child Language.
[9] Jeffrey L. Elman,et al. Finding Structure in Time , 1990, Cogn. Sci..
[10] Jennifer E. Arnold,et al. Heaviness vs. newness: The effects of structural complexity and discourse status on constituent ordering , 2015 .
[11] Geoffrey E. Hinton. Learning and Applying Contextual Constraints in Sentence Comprehension , 1991 .
[12] Zenzi M. Griffin,et al. The persistence of structural priming: transient activation or implicit learning? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[13] D. Bishop,et al. CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE What Causes Specific Language Impairment in Children? , 2022 .
[14] K. Bock,et al. Framing sentences , 1990, Cognition.
[15] G. Dell,et al. Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.
[16] C. Norbury,et al. Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders. , 2005, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[17] M. MacDonald. Distributional Information in Language Comprehension, Production, and Acquisition: Three Puzzles and a Moral , 2013 .
[18] James L. McClelland,et al. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.
[19] G. Altmann,et al. Incremental interpretation at verbs: restricting the domain of subsequent reference , 1999, Cognition.
[20] F. Chang. Symbolically speaking: a connectionist model of sentence production , 2002 .
[21] S. Carey,et al. Language deficits after apparent clinical recovery from childhood aphasia , 1979, Annals of neurology.
[22] F. Chang,et al. “Long before short” preference in the production of a head-final language , 2001, Cognition.
[23] Laine Matti,et al. Modelling anomia by the discrete two-stage word production architecture , 1998, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[24] M. Schwartz,et al. Semantic Factors in Verb Retrieval: An Effect of Complexity , 1998, Brain and Language.
[25] Robin K. Morris,et al. Processing Subject and Object Relative Clauses: Evidence from Eye Movements , 2002 .
[26] Robert J. Hartsuiker,et al. Does verb bias modulate syntactic priming? , 2010, Cognition.
[27] Don L. Scarborough,et al. Frequency and Repetition Effects in Lexical Memory. , 1977 .
[28] R. Levy. Expectation-based syntactic comprehension , 2008, Cognition.
[29] J. Elman. Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small , 1993, Cognition.
[30] Franklin Chang,et al. Learning to order words: A connectionist model of heavy NP shift and accessibility effects in Japanese and English , 2009 .
[31] G. Dell,et al. Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers. , 1997, Psychological review.
[32] V. M. Holmes,et al. Planning units and syntax in sentence production , 1978, Cognition.
[33] G. Dell,et al. Persistent structural priming from language comprehension to language production , 2007, Cognition.
[34] Katherine A. DeLong,et al. Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[35] M. Pickering,et al. Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[36] Paola Cipriani,et al. The Italian Determiner System in Normal Acquisition, Specific Language Impairment, and Childhood Aphasia , 2001, Brain and Language.
[37] Sudha Arunachalam,et al. Meaning from syntax: Evidence from 2-year-olds , 2010, Cognition.
[38] Kevin Durkin,et al. Specific language impairment and school outcomes. I: identifying and explaining variability at the end of compulsory education. , 2009, International journal of language & communication disorders.
[39] Jordan Grafman,et al. Neuronal Plasticity: Building a Bridge from the Laboratory to the Clinic , 1999, Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences.
[40] Caroline F. Rowland,et al. Comprehension of Argument Structure and Semantic Roles: Evidence from English-Learning Children and the Forced-Choice Pointing Paradigm , 2011, Cogn. Sci..
[41] Kathryn Bock,et al. Closed-class immanence in sentence production , 1989, Cognition.
[42] Gary M. Oppenheim,et al. The dark side of incremental learning: A model of cumulative semantic interference during lexical access in speech production , 2010, Cognition.
[43] Ayumi Matsuo,et al. Japanese two-year-olds use morphosyntax to learn novel verb meanings* , 2011, Journal of Child Language.
[44] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. Reassessing Working Memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996) , 2002 .
[45] Kara D. Federmeier. Thinking ahead: the role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[46] P. O'Seaghdha,et al. Phrasal Ordering Constraints in Sentence Production: Phrase Length and Verb Disposition in Heavy-NP Shift , 1998 .
[47] J. K. Bock. Syntactic persistence in language production , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.
[48] B. Woods,et al. Changing patterns of childhood aphasia , 1978, Annals of neurology.
[49] J. Morton,et al. Effect of word transitional probability on phoneme identification , 1976 .
[50] Sylvia Yuan,et al. “Really? She Blicked the Baby?” , 2009, Psychological science.
[51] Julie C. Sedivy,et al. Subject Terms: Linguistics Language Eyes & eyesight Cognition & reasoning , 1995 .
[52] T. Rogers,et al. Lichtheim 2: Synthesizing Aphasia and the Neural Basis of Language in a Neurocomputational Model of the Dual Dorsal-Ventral Language Pathways , 2011, Neuron.
[53] G. Conti-Ramsden,et al. Verb schema use and input dependence in 5-year-old children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). , 2006, International journal of language & communication disorders.
[54] J. Fodor,et al. The Psychology of Language: An Introduction to Psycholinguistics and Generative Grammar , 1976 .
[55] Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al. Linking production and comprehension processes: The case of relative clauses , 2009, Cognition.
[56] Jean K. Gordon,et al. Learning to divide the labor: an account of deficits in light and heavy verb production , 2003, Cogn. Sci..
[57] T. Shallice,et al. Deep Dyslexia: A Case Study of , 1993 .
[58] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[59] Dorothy V.M. Bishop,et al. Grammatical Difficulties in Children with Specific Language Impairment: Is Learning Deficient? , 2011, Human Development.
[60] Silvia P. Gennari,et al. Animacy and competition in relative clause production: A cross-linguistic investigation , 2012, Cognitive Psychology.
[61] D. Plaut,et al. Connectionist Modeling of Relearning and Generalization in Acquired Dyslexic Patients , 1999 .
[62] Gary S Dell,et al. Limits on learning phonotactic constraints from recent production experience. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[63] G. Dell,et al. Becoming syntactic. , 2006, Psychological review.
[64] Michael S. C. Thomas,et al. Are developmental disorders like cases of adult brain damage? Implications from connectionist modelling , 2002, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[65] Michael P. Wilson,et al. Memory for Syntax Despite Amnesia , 2008, Psychological science.
[66] R. Brubaker. Models for the perception of speech and visual form: Weiant Wathen-Dunn, ed.: Cambridge, Mass., The M.I.T. Press, I–X, 470 pages , 1968 .
[67] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Phonology and syntax in specific language impairment: Evidence from a connectionist model , 2003, Brain and Language.
[68] Falk Huettig,et al. Prediction during language processing is a piece of cake--but only for skilled producers. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[69] Nick Chater,et al. Language Learning From Positive Evidence, Reconsidered: A Simplicity-Based Approach , 2013, Top. Cogn. Sci..
[70] Michael J. Spivey,et al. The Cambridge Handbook of Psycholinguistics , 2012 .
[71] James L. McClelland,et al. Learning the structure of event sequences. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[72] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Selective difficulties with spoken nouns and written verbs: A single case study , 2002, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[73] T. Jaeger,et al. Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime’s prediction error given both prior and recent experience , 2013, Cognition.
[74] E. Newport,et al. Learning at a distance I. Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.
[75] Franklin Chang,et al. A connectionist account of the acquisition and processing of relative clauses , 2011 .