Behavioral consequences of selective damage to frontal pole and posterior cingulate cortices

Significance Frontal pole cortex (FPC) refers to the most anterior part of prefrontal cortex, a region that is highly developed in anthropoid primates. However, because of technical difficulties in studying this area, its role in primate cognition had remained largely unknown. We studied effects of selective bilateral lesions within FPC on monkeys’ cognitive flexibility. FPC lesion did not impair the performance in well-learned cognitively demanding tasks. However, FPC-lesioned monkeys remained more focused than control monkeys in exploiting the current task when they faced newly introduced interruptions by a simple secondary task or free rewards. This unique pattern of behavioral changes in FPC-lesioned monkeys suggests that FPC is involved in redistribution of cognitive resources from the current task to novel opportunities. Frontal pole cortex (FPC) and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) have close neuroanatomical connections, and imaging studies have shown coactivation or codeactivation of these brain regions during performance of certain tasks. However, they are among the least well-understood regions of the primate brain. One reason for this is that the consequences of selective bilateral lesions to either structure have not previously been studied in any primate species. We studied the effects of circumscribed bilateral lesions to FPC or PCC on monkeys’ ability to perform an analog of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and related tasks. In contrast to lesions in other prefrontal regions, neither posttraining FPC nor PCC lesions impaired animals’ abilities to follow the rule switches that frequently occurred within the WCST task. However, FPC lesions were not without effect, because they augmented the ability of animals to adjust cognitive control after experiencing high levels of conflict (whereas PCC lesions did not have any effect). In addition, FPC-lesioned monkeys were more successful than controls or PCC-lesioned animals at remembering the relevant rule across experimentally imposed distractions involving either an intervening secondary task or a surprising delivery of free reward. Although prefrontal cortex posterior to FPC is specialized for mediating efficient goal-directed behavior to maximally exploit reward opportunities from ongoing tasks, our data led us to suggest that FPC is, instead, specialized for disengaging executive control from the current task and redistributing it to novel sources of reward to explore new opportunities/goals.

[1]  M. Platt,et al.  Risk-sensitive neurons in macaque posterior cingulate cortex , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  S. Luck,et al.  The Construct of Attention in Schizophrenia , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[3]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[4]  Hsin-Hao Yu,et al.  Cortical input to the frontal pole of the marmoset monkey. , 2011, Cerebral Cortex.

[5]  J. Price,et al.  Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex , 2003, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[6]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain , 2007, Nature.

[7]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  How Green Is the Grass on the Other Side? Frontopolar Cortex and the Evidence in Favor of Alternative Courses of Action , 2009, Neuron.

[8]  T. Egner Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[9]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Relational Integration during Reasoning , 2001, NeuroImage.

[10]  Angela R. Laird,et al.  Cytoarchitecture, probability maps and functions of the human frontal pole , 2014, NeuroImage.

[11]  P. Glimcher,et al.  The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  C. Frith,et al.  Differential involvement of regions of rostral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann area 10) in time- and event-based prospective memory. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[13]  Etienne Koechlin,et al.  Foundations of human reasoning in the prefrontal cortex , 2014, Science.

[14]  D. Pandya,et al.  Efferent Association Pathways from the Rostral Prefrontal Cortex in the Macaque Monkey , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  D. Stuss,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with focal frontal and posterior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  J. Jacobs,et al.  Regional dendritic and spine variation in human cerebral cortex: a quantitative golgi study. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  K. Davis,et al.  Connectivity-based parcellation of the human frontal polar cortex , 2014, Brain Structure and Function.

[18]  Steven Laureys,et al.  Posterior cingulate, precuneal and retrosplenial cortices: cytology and components of the neural network correlates of consciousness. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[19]  John M. Pearson,et al.  Posterior cingulate cortex: adapting behavior to a changing world , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  G. Orban,et al.  Default Mode of Brain Function in Monkeys , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  M. Watanabe,et al.  Are there internal thought processes in the monkey?—Default brain activity in humans and nonhuman primates , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[22]  D. Amaral,et al.  Macaque monkey retrosplenial cortex: I. Three‐dimensional and cytoarchitectonic organization , 2000, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[23]  John M. Pearson,et al.  Neurons in Posterior Cingulate Cortex Signal Exploratory Decisions in a Dynamic Multioption Choice Task , 2009, Current Biology.

[24]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Conflict-induced behavioural adjustment: a clue to the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Organization of cognitive control within the lateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. , 2009, Archives of general psychiatry.

[26]  Marcello G P Rosa,et al.  Subcortical projections to the frontal pole in the marmoset monkey , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[27]  G. V. Van Hoesen,et al.  Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: a comparative study of area 10. , 2001, American journal of physical anthropology.

[28]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  The Essential Role of Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex in Conflict-Induced Executive Control Adjustment , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition , 1999, Nature.

[30]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[31]  Philip G. F. Browning,et al.  Dissociable Components of Rule-Guided Behavior Depend on Distinct Medial and Prefrontal Regions , 2009, Science.

[32]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[33]  David Badre,et al.  Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro–caudal organization of the frontal lobes , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  Benjamin Y. Hayden,et al.  Posterior Cingulate Cortex Mediates Outcome-Contingent Allocation of Behavior , 2008, Neuron.

[35]  P. Dayan,et al.  Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans , 2006, Nature.

[36]  C. Frith,et al.  Recruitment of lateral rostral prefrontal cortex in spontaneous and task-related thoughts , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[37]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[38]  P. B. Cipolloni,et al.  Cytoarchitecture and cortical connections of the posterior cingulate and adjacent somatosensory fields in the rhesus monkey , 2004, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[39]  Paul W. Burgess,et al.  Specialization of the Rostral Prefrontal Cortex for Distinct Analogy Processes , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  Katsuyuki Sakai,et al.  Is the Prefrontal Cortex Necessary for Establishing Cognitive Sets? , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  B. Vogt,et al.  Architecture and neurocytology of monkey cingulate gyrus , 2005, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[43]  D L Braff,et al.  Increased distractibility in schizophrenic patients. Electrophysiologic and behavioral evidence. , 1990, Archives of general psychiatry.

[44]  N. Cozzarelli PNAS Early Edition. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  D. Sharp,et al.  The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  M. Buckley,et al.  Essential functions of primate frontopolar cortex in cognition , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[47]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[48]  A. Owen,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  T. Shallice,et al.  The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  D. Pandya,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[51]  S. Pollmann Anterior Prefrontal Contributions to Implicit Attention Control , 2012, Brain sciences.

[52]  Carter Wendelken,et al.  Transitive Inference: Distinct Contributions of Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex and the Hippocampus , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[53]  H. Damasio,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance as a measure of frontal lobe damage. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[54]  J. Reichenbach,et al.  The special involvement of the rostrolateral prefrontal cortex in planning abilities: An event-related fMRI study with the Tower of London paradigm , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[56]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Function and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[57]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Damage to the Fronto-Polar Cortex Is Associated with Impaired Multitasking , 2008, PloS one.

[58]  Sarah R. Heilbronner,et al.  Decision Salience Signals in Posterior Cingulate Cortex , 2011, Front. Neurosci..

[59]  S. Wise,et al.  Frontal pole cortex: encoding ends at the end of the endbrain , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Mnemonic Function of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Conflict-Induced Behavioral Adjustment , 2007, Science.

[61]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Prefrontal Cell Activities Related to Monkeys' Success and Failure in Adapting to Rule Changes in a Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Analog , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  G. V. Van Hoesen,et al.  Frontal granular cortex input to the cingulate (M3), supplementary (M2) and primary (M1) motor cortices in the rhesus monkey , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[63]  Giuseppe Pagnoni,et al.  A comparison of resting-state brain activity in humans and chimpanzees , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[64]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Cognitive Control Functions of Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Macaque Monkeys Performing a Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Analog , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[65]  Todd B. Parrish,et al.  The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention , 2003, NeuroImage.

[66]  E. Awh,et al.  Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[67]  K. Hikosaka,et al.  Default Mode of Brain Activity Demonstrated by Positron Emission Tomography Imaging in Awake Monkeys: Higher Rest-Related than Working Memory-Related Activity in Medial Cortical Areas , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[68]  J. Fuster Upper processing stages of the perception–action cycle , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[69]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Evaluating self-generated information: anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[70]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Response to Comment on "Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought" , 2007, Science.