Wordform Similarity Increases With Semantic Similarity: An Analysis of 100 Languages
暂无分享,去创建一个
Edward Gibson | Steve Piantadosi | Isabelle Dautriche | Kyle Mahowald | S. Piantadosi | E. Gibson | Kyle Mahowald | Isabelle Dautriche
[1] Abdellah Fourtassi,et al. A corpus-based evaluation method for Distributional Semantic Models , 2013, ACL.
[2] Kong-On Kim,et al. Sound symbolism in Korean , 1977, Journal of Linguistics.
[3] Scott A. Jackson,et al. High functional load inhibits phonological contrast loss: A corpus study , 2013, Cognition.
[4] A. Christophe,et al. Learning novel phonological neighbors: Syntactic category matters , 2015, Cognition.
[5] R. Paget. The Origin of Speech , 1927, Nature.
[6] Keith Johnson,et al. Why reduce? Phonological neighborhood density and phonetic reduction in spontaneous speech , 2012 .
[7] Julie C. Sedivy,et al. Subject Terms: Linguistics Language Eyes & eyesight Cognition & reasoning , 1995 .
[8] Zellig S. Harris,et al. Distributional Structure , 1954 .
[9] Jeremy Goslin,et al. PhonItalia: a phonological lexicon for Italian , 2014, Behavior research methods.
[10] C. Habel,et al. Language , 1931, NeuroImage.
[11] Peter Graff,et al. Communicative Efficiency in the Lexicon , 2014 .
[12] Edward Gibson,et al. Word Forms Are Structured for Efficient Use , 2018, Cogn. Sci..
[13] Dekang Lin,et al. An Information-Theoretic Definition of Similarity , 1998, ICML.
[14] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. Sound–meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[15] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Lexique 2 : A new French lexical database , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[16] Hans Peter Luhn,et al. The Automatic Creation of Literature Abstracts , 1958, IBM J. Res. Dev..
[17] B. Bergen. The Psychological Reality of Phonaesthemes , 2004 .
[18] B. Lindblom. Phonetic Universals in Vowel Systems , 1986 .
[19] T. Landauer,et al. A Solution to Plato's Problem: The Latent Semantic Analysis Theory of Acquisition, Induction, and Representation of Knowledge. , 1997 .
[20] Scott McDonald,et al. Filled pauses and their status in the mental lexicon , 2001, DiSS.
[21] Petr Sojka,et al. Software Framework for Topic Modelling with Large Corpora , 2010 .
[22] Shoko Hamano. The sound-symbolic system of Japanese , 2000 .
[23] Joseph Paul Stemberger,et al. Neighbourhood effects on error rates in speech production , 2004, Brain and Language.
[24] Willem H. Zuidema. Optimal Communication in a Noisy and Heterogeneous Environment , 2003, ECAL.
[25] Charles Kemp,et al. Kinship Categories Across Languages Reflect General Communicative Principles , 2012, Science.
[26] Monica Tamariz,et al. Exploring systematicity between phonological and context-cooccurrence representations of the mental lexicon , 2008 .
[27] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. Arbitrariness, Iconicity, and Systematicity in Language , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[28] H. Storkel,et al. Differentiating phonotactic probability and neighborhood density in adult word learning. , 2006, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[29] Claude E. Shannon,et al. The mathematical theory of communication , 1950 .
[30] Dan Jurafsky,et al. Effects of disfluencies, predictability, and utterance position on word form variation in English conversation. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[31] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. From sound to syntax: phonological constraints on children's lexical categorization of new words* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[32] M. H. Kelly,et al. Using sound to solve syntactic problems: the role of phonology in grammatical category assignments. , 1992, Psychological review.
[33] Michael S Vitevitch,et al. The facilitative influence of phonological similarity and neighborhood frequency in speech production in younger and older adults , 2003, Memory & cognition.
[34] Austin F. Frank,et al. Speaking Rationally: Uniform Information Density as an Optimal Strategy for Language Production , 2008 .
[35] C. Spence,et al. “Bouba” and “Kiki” in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape–sound matches, but different shape–taste matches to Westerners , 2013, Cognition.
[36] Sarah C. Creel,et al. Heeding the voice of experience: The role of talker variation in lexical access , 2008, Cognition.
[37] S. Kirby,et al. Cultural selection for learnability: Three principles underlying the view that language adapts to be learnable , 2006 .
[38] Philippe Schlenker,et al. Event representations constrain the structure of language: Sign language as a window into universally accessible linguistic biases , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[39] Alice Turk,et al. The Smooth Signal Redundancy Hypothesis: A Functional Explanation for Relationships between Redundancy, Prosodic Prominence, and Duration in Spontaneous Speech , 2004, Language and speech.
[40] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Subtlex-pl: subtitle-based word frequency estimates for Polish , 2014, Behavior Research Methods.
[41] Roger Levy,et al. Speakers optimize information density through syntactic reduction , 2006, NIPS.
[42] H. Storkel,et al. The independent effects of phonotactic probability and neighbourhood density on lexical acquisition by preschool children , 2011, Language and cognitive processes.
[43] Steven Pinker,et al. Language learnability and language development , 1985 .
[44] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. The arbitrariness of the sign: learning advantages from the structure of the vocabulary. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[45] Sarah C. Creel. Preschoolers' use of talker information in on-line comprehension. , 2012, Child development.
[46] D. Maurer,et al. Synesthesia: a new approach to understanding the development of perception. , 2013, Developmental psychology.
[47] G. Miller,et al. Cognitive science. , 1981, Science.
[48] Steven T Piantadosi,et al. Word lengths are optimized for efficient communication , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[49] Ferdinand de Saussure. Course in General Linguistics , 1916 .
[50] Edward Flemming. Contrast and Perceptual Distinctiveness , 2003 .
[51] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. Integration of multiple probabilistic cues in syntax acquisition , 2008 .
[52] George A. Miller,et al. WordNet: A Lexical Database for English , 1995, HLT.
[53] Sang Joon Kim,et al. A Mathematical Theory of Communication , 2006 .
[54] Benjamin Bergen,et al. Finding Non-Arbitrary Form-Meaning Systematicity Using String-Metric Learning for Kernel Regression , 2016, ACL.
[55] George Kingsley Zipf,et al. Human behavior and the principle of least effort , 1949 .
[56] G. Altmann,et al. Incremental interpretation at verbs: restricting the domain of subsequent reference , 1999, Cognition.
[57] Simon Kirby,et al. Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[58] Simon Kirby,et al. How arbitrary is language? , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[59] S. Kita,et al. The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[60] M. H. Kelly,et al. Phonological information for grammatical category assignments , 1991 .
[61] N. Chater,et al. Simplicity: a unifying principle in cognitive science? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.