Subliminal visual oddball stimuli evoke a P300 component

OBJECTIVE To provide evidence that a P300 component can be elicited by subliminal stimuli in an oddball paradigm. METHODS The words LEFT and RIGHT were presented in a frequent-rare ratio (80-20%), counterbalanced between subjects. Stimuli were presented at the objective detection threshold (d'=0, via unmasked 1 ms presentations), a stringent measure for detecting any conscious perception. RESULTS A significantly larger amplitude component was found for rare vs. frequent stimulus presentations across electrodes Fz, Cz, and Pz using both a broad 200-900 ms window (F(1,27)=5.75, P<0.012, eta(2)=0.18; one-tailed), and a more narrow 400-760 ms window defined using principal component analysis (F(1,27)=10.10, P<0.002, eta(2)=0.27; one-tailed). No significant component latency effects were found. An analysis of the conscious perception index (d') and the oddball effect (rare-frequent amplitude difference) revealed a negative relationship, further supporting the contention that conscious perception does not account for the finding, and suggesting that any conscious stimulus detection may inhibit this subliminal effect. CONCLUSIONS Results provide evidence that an endogenous component can be elicited by undetectable subliminal stimuli in an oddball paradigm. Implications are discussed for comparing conscious and unconscious information processing, unconscious learning, and the measurement of ERPs to subliminal stimuli.

[1]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Activation by marginally perceptible ("subliminal") stimuli: dissociation of unconscious from conscious cognition. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[2]  J. Polich,et al.  Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review , 1995, Biological Psychology.

[3]  Stephen J. Boies,et al.  Components of attention. , 1971 .

[4]  B. Dosher The response-window regression method—Some problematic assumptions: Comment on Draine and Greenwald (1998). , 1998 .

[5]  E. Reingold,et al.  Using direct and indirect measures to study perception without awareness , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[7]  H. Shevrin,et al.  Visual Evoked Response Correlates of Unconscious Mental Processes , 1968, Science.

[8]  Thomas H. Carr,et al.  Task-induced strategies and near-threshold priming: Conscious influences on unconscious perception. , 1989 .

[9]  E. Donchin,et al.  Psychophysiology : systems, processes, and applications , 1987 .

[10]  Daniel Holender,et al.  Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal , 1986, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[11]  E. Bernat,et al.  Brain Indices of Nonconscious Associative Learning , 1997, Consciousness and Cognition.

[12]  E. Reingold,et al.  On demonstrating unconscious perception: comment on Draine and Greenwald (1998) , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[13]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Discrimination and learning without awareness: a methodological survey and evaluation. , 1960, Psychological review.

[14]  A. Öhman,et al.  Preparedness and preattentive associative learning: Electrodermal conditioning to masked stimuli. , 1995 .

[15]  E. Bernat,et al.  Further evidence for unconscious learning: preliminary support for the conditioning of facial EMG to subliminal stimuli. , 1999, Journal of psychiatric research.

[16]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Bioelectrical echoes from evaluative categorizations: I. A late positive brain potential that varies as a function of trait negativity and extremity. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  W. Andrew The vertebrate visual system , 1957 .

[18]  P. Merikle,et al.  Distinguishing conscious from unconscious perceptual processes. , 1986, Canadian journal of psychology.

[19]  W. Pritchard Psychophysiology of P300. , 1981, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  D. M. Green,et al.  Detection and recognition. , 1978 .

[21]  John A. Groeger,et al.  Qualitatively Different Effects of Undetected and Unidentified Auditory Primes , 1988 .

[22]  M. Rugg,et al.  Electrophysiology of Mind: Event-Related Brain Potentials and Cognition , 1995 .

[23]  P. Merikle,et al.  Priming with and without awareness , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  C. Izard,et al.  Four systems for emotion activation: cognitive and noncognitive processes. , 1993, Psychological review.

[25]  L. Jacoby A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory , 1991 .

[26]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Additivity of nonconscious affect: combined effects of priming and exposure. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  Handbook of cognition and emotion , 1999 .

[28]  J. Polich P300 from a passive auditory paradigm. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[29]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[30]  E. Donchin,et al.  Cognitive Psychophysiology and Human Information Processing , 1986 .

[31]  Correcting for measurement error in detecting unconscious cognition: comment on Draine and Greenwald (1998) , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[32]  N. Fox,et al.  If it's not left, it's right. Electroencephalograph asymmetry and the development of emotion. , 1991, The American psychologist.

[33]  A G Greenwald,et al.  Unconscious processing of dichoptically masked words , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[34]  G A Miller,et al.  How we think about cognition, emotion, and biology in psychopathology. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[35]  W. Sommer,et al.  Awareness of P300-related cognitive processes: a signal detection approach. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[36]  U Dimberg,et al.  Nonconscious associative learning: Pavlovian conditioning of skin conductance responses to masked fear-relevant facial stimuli. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[37]  E. Bernat,et al.  Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[38]  R. Chapman,et al.  EP Component Identification and Measurement by Principal Components-Analysis , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[39]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 topography and modality effects from a single-stimulus paradigm. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[40]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  The psychophysics of subliminal perception , 1986, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[41]  J. Kihlstrom The cognitive unconscious. , 1987, Science.

[42]  E. Reingold,et al.  Theory and measurement in the study of unconscious processes , 1990 .

[43]  M. Rugg,et al.  Electrophysiology of Mind. , 1996 .

[44]  Vic Barnett,et al.  Outliers in Statistical Data , 1980 .

[45]  I GOLDIAMOND,et al.  Indicators of perception. I. Subliminal perception, subception, unconscious perception: an analysis in terms of psychophysical indicator methodology. , 1958, Psychological bulletin.

[46]  M. Coles,et al.  Event-related brain potentials: An introduction. , 1995 .

[47]  M Brázdil,et al.  Effect of subthreshold target stimuli on event-related potentials. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[48]  Joseph E LeDoux Emotion: clues from the brain. , 1995, Annual review of psychology.

[49]  W. J. Williams,et al.  Conscious and nonconscious processes: an ERP index of an anticipatory response in a conditioning paradigm using visually masked stimuli. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[50]  K C Klauer,et al.  An unbiased errors-in-variables approach to detecting unconscious cognition. , 1998, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[51]  Howard Shevrin,et al.  The psychological unconscious: A necessary assumption for all psychological theory? , 1980 .

[52]  J. Andreassi,et al.  Amplitude changes in the visual evoked cortical potential with backward masking. , 1976, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[53]  H. Shevrin,et al.  The Mediation of Intentional Judgments by Unconscious Perceptions: The Influences of Task Strategy, Task Preference, Word Meaning, and Motivation , 1993, Consciousness and Cognition.

[54]  T. Demiralp,et al.  The effects of subthreshold visual stimulation on P300 response , 1997, Neuroreport.

[55]  E. Donchin,et al.  Probability effects on stimulus evaluation and response processes. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[56]  W. J. Williams,et al.  Conscious and Unconscious Processes: Psychodynamic, Cognitive, and Neurophysiological Convergences , 1996 .