Investigations of a complex, realistic task: Intentional, unsystematic, and exhaustive experimenters

This study examines how students' experimentation with a virtual environment contrib- utes to their understanding of a complex, realistic inquiry problem. We designed a week-long, technolo- gy-enhanced inquiry unit on car collisions. The unit uses new technologies to log students' experimentation choices. Physics students (n ¼ 148) in six diverse high schools studied the unit and responded to pretests, posttests, and embedded assessments. We scored students' experimentation using four methods: total number of trials, variability of variable choices, propensity to vary one variable at a time, and coherence between investigation goals and experimentation methods. Students made moderate, significant overall pretest to posttest gains on physics understanding. Coherence was a strong predictor of learning, controlling for pretest scores and the other experimentation measures. We identify three categories of experimenters (intentional, unsystematic, and exhaustive) and illustrate these categories with examples. The findings suggest that students must combine disciplinary knowledge of the investi- gation with intentional investigation of the inquiry questions in order to understand the nature of the variables. Mechanically executing well-established experimentation procedures (such as varying one variable at a time or comprehensively exploring the experimentation space) is less likely to lead students to valuable insights about complex tasks. Our proposed categories extend and refine previous efforts to categorize experimenters by linking scientific procedures with understanding of the science discipline.

[1]  F. Giambona STATISTICAL METHODS FOR THE SOCIAL SCIENCES , 2013 .

[2]  Noemi Waight,et al.  From scientific practice to high school science classrooms: Transfer of scientific technologies and realizations of authentic inquiry , 2011 .

[3]  Janet F. Carlson,et al.  The relative effects and equity of inquiry‐based and commonplace science teaching on students' knowledge, reasoning, and argumentation , 2009 .

[4]  Marios Papaevripidou,et al.  Effects of experimenting with physical and virtual manipulatives on students' conceptual understanding in heat and temperature , 2008 .

[5]  M. Linn,et al.  Assessing Knowledge Integration in Science: Construct, Measures, and Evidence , 2008 .

[6]  Marcia C. Linn,et al.  Targeted Support for Using Technology-Enhanced Science Inquiry Modules , 2008 .

[7]  Brian J. Reiser,et al.  Software-realized inquiry support for cultivating a disciplinary stance , 2008 .

[8]  Corinne Zimmerman The Development of Scientific Thinking Skills in Elementary and Middle School. , 2007 .

[9]  Jerry Wellington,et al.  America's lab report: Investigations in high school science , 2007 .

[10]  D. Kuhn,et al.  Direct instruction vs. discovery: The long view , 2007 .

[11]  H. Schweingruber,et al.  TAKING SCIENCE TO SCHOOL: LEARNING AND TEACHING SCIENCE IN GRADES K-8 , 2007 .

[12]  Lara M. Triona,et al.  Hands on What? The Relative Effectiveness of Physical versus Virtual Materials in an Engineering Design Project by Middle School Children , 2007 .

[13]  Jennifer L. Chiu,et al.  Teaching and Assessing Knowledge Integration in Science , 2006, Science.

[14]  Barbara C. Buckley,et al.  Using Log Files to Track Students' Model-based Inquiry in Science , 2006, ICLS.

[15]  Yael Kali,et al.  Collaborative knowledge building using the Design Principles Database , 2006, Int. J. Comput. Support. Collab. Learn..

[16]  Marcia C. Linn,et al.  Science Education: Integrating Views of Learning and Instruction , 2006 .

[17]  H. Schweingruber,et al.  America's lab report : investigations in high school science , 2006 .

[18]  R. Marx,et al.  Design‐based science and student learning , 2004 .

[19]  Milena K. Nigam,et al.  The Equivalence of Learning Paths in Early Science Instruction: Effects of Direct Instruction and Discovery Learning , 2022 .

[20]  R. Millar,et al.  Reasoning from data: How students collect and interpret data in science investigations , 2004 .

[21]  Marcia C. Linn,et al.  Internet Environments for Science Education , 2004 .

[22]  E. Soloway,et al.  Creating a Framework for Research on Systemic Technology Innovations , 2004 .

[23]  Jianwei Zhang,et al.  Supporting scientific discovery learning in a simulation environment , 2003, J. Comput. Assist. Learn..

[24]  J. Levin Computers, teachers, peers: Science learning partners , 2003 .

[25]  R. Mistretta,et al.  "Principles and Standards for School Mathematics" in the Classroom. , 2001 .

[26]  Iris Tabak,et al.  BGuILE: Stragtegic and conceptual scaffolds for scientific inquiry in biology classrooms , 2001 .

[27]  Joseph L. Polman,et al.  Designing Project-Based Science: Connecting Learners Through Guided Inquiry (Ways of Knowing in Science Series) , 1999 .

[28]  D. Klahr,et al.  All other things being equal: acquisition and transfer of the control of variables strategy. , 1999, Child development.

[29]  J. Frederiksen,et al.  Inquiry, Modeling, and Metacognition: Making Science Accessible to All Students , 1998 .

[30]  Joseph Krajcik,et al.  Instructional, Curricular, and Technological Supports for Inquiry in Science Classrooms , 1998 .

[31]  L. Schauble,et al.  The development of scientific reasoning in knowledge-rich contexts. , 1996 .

[32]  B. Koslowski Theory and Evidence: The Development of Scientific Reasoning , 1996 .

[33]  Bruce D. Burns,et al.  The Impact of Goal Specificity on Strategy Use and the Acquisition of Problem Structure , 1996, Cogn. Sci..

[34]  E. Soloway,et al.  A Collaborative Model for Helping Middle Grade Science Teachers Learn Project-Based Instruction , 1994, The Elementary School Journal.

[35]  Kevin Dunbar,et al.  Concept Discovery in a Scientific Domain , 1993, Cogn. Sci..

[36]  B. White ThinkerTools: Causal Models, Conceptual Change, and Science Education , 1993 .

[37]  D. Klahr,et al.  Heuristics for Scientific Experimentation: A Developmental Study , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[38]  M. Linn,et al.  Teaching thermodynamics to middle school students: What are appropriate cognitive demands? , 1991 .

[39]  L. Schauble,et al.  Students' transition from an engineering model to a science model of experimentation , 1991 .

[40]  Lillian C. McDermott,et al.  Millikan Lecture 1990: What we teach and what is learned—Closing the gap , 1991 .

[41]  Gaea Leinhardt,et al.  Functions, Graphs, and Graphing: Tasks, Learning, and Teaching , 1990 .

[42]  L. Schauble,et al.  Belief revision in children: the role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. , 1990, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[43]  Susan Carey,et al.  `An experiment is when you try it and see if it works': a study of grade 7 students' understanding of the construction of scientific knowledge , 1989 .

[44]  David Klahr,et al.  Dual Space Search During Scientific Reasoning , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[45]  Lillian C. McDermott,et al.  Student difficulties in connecting graphs and physics: Examples from kinematics , 1987 .

[46]  Marcia C. Linn,et al.  Is it formal if it's not physics? (the influence of content on formal reasoning) , 1983 .

[47]  E. Phelps,et al.  The development of problem-solving strategies. , 1982, Advances in child development and behavior.

[48]  L. McDermott,et al.  Investigation of student understanding of the concept of acceleration in one dimension , 1981 .

[49]  L. McDermott,et al.  Investigation of student understanding of the concept of velocity in one dimension , 1980 .

[50]  J. E. Tschirgi,et al.  Sensible reasoning: A hypothesis about hypotheses. , 1980 .

[51]  M. Linn Is It Formal If It's Not Physics? (The Influence of Laboratory and Naturalistic Content on Formal Reasoning). , 1980 .

[52]  A. Agresti,et al.  Statistical Methods for the Social Sciences , 1979 .

[53]  R. Siegler,et al.  Acquisition of formal scientific reasoning by 10- and 13-year-olds: Designing a factorial experiment. , 1975 .

[54]  Ruth M. Beard,et al.  The Growth of Logical Thinking , 1972 .