Perceived regularity of a texture is influenced by the regularity of a surrounding texture
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Benoit Cottereau,et al. Dynamics of perceptual decisions about symmetry in visual cortex , 2018, NeuroImage.
[2] J. Gibson. The perception of the visual world , 1951 .
[3] Brian Roberts,et al. The Roles of Inducer Size and Distance in the Ebbinghaus Illusion (Titchener Circles) , 2005, Perception.
[4] R. F Hess,et al. How many positions can we perceptually encode, one or many? , 2003, Vision Research.
[5] M A Georgeson,et al. The effect of spatial adaptation on perceived contrast. , 1985, Spatial vision.
[6] P. Green,et al. Measuring perceived differences in surface texture due to changes in higher order statistics. , 2010, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[7] Colin W G Clifford,et al. The tilt illusion: Phenomenology and functional implications , 2014, Vision Research.
[8] J. Gibson. The perception of visual surfaces. , 1950, The American journal of psychology.
[9] David C. Knill,et al. Surface orientation from texture: ideal observers, generic observers and the information content of texture cues , 1998, Vision Research.
[10] Paul M Bays,et al. Obligatory encoding of task-irrelevant features depletes working memory resources. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[11] E. G. Heinemann,et al. Simultaneous brightness induction as a function of inducing and test-field luminances. , 1955, Journal of experimental psychology.
[12] N Ginsburg,et al. Effect of Item Arrangement on Perceived Numerosity: Randomness vs Regularity , 1976, Perceptual and motor skills.
[13] Johan Wagemans,et al. Quantifying density cues in grouping displays , 2016, Vision Research.
[14] Peter A. van der Helm,et al. Weber-Fechner behavior in symmetry perception? , 2010 .
[15] Johan Wagemans,et al. Spatial arrangement in texture discrimination and texture segregation , 2013, i-Perception.
[16] Roger W Remington,et al. Segmentation by depth does not always facilitate visual search. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[17] Steven C. Dakin,et al. The common perceptual metric for human discrimination of number and density , 2011 .
[18] Denis G. Pelli,et al. ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.
[19] Mark W. Cannon,et al. Spatial interactions in apparent contrast: Individual differences in enhancement and suppression effects , 1993, Vision Research.
[20] Takahiro Kawabe,et al. Pattern randomness aftereffect , 2013, Scientific Reports.
[21] Frederick A A Kingdom,et al. Simultaneous density contrast is bidirectional. , 2016, Journal of vision.
[22] S. Klein,et al. The simultaneous spatial frequency shift: a dissociation between the detection and perception of gratings. , 1974, Vision research.
[23] C. Baker,et al. Texture density adaptation can be bidirectional. , 2017, Journal of vision.
[24] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[25] D. Mackay,et al. Lateral Interaction between Neural Channels sensitive to Texture Density? , 1973, Nature.
[26] F. Kingdom,et al. Aftereffect of perceived regularity. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[27] Robert Tibshirani,et al. An Introduction to the Bootstrap , 1994 .
[28] Lewis D. Griffin,et al. An absolute interval scale of order for point patterns , 2014, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.
[29] F. Kingdom,et al. Different symmetries, different mechanisms. , 2018, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[30] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[31] A. Norcia,et al. Representation of Maximally Regular Textures in Human Visual Cortex , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[32] Hiroshi Ban,et al. A non-device-specific approach to display characterization based on linear, nonlinear, and hybrid search algorithms. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[33] Steven M. Thurman,et al. A comparison of form processing involved in the perception of biological and nonbiological movements , 2016, Journal of vision.
[34] Norma V. Graham,et al. Beyond multiple pattern analyzers modeled as linear filters (as classical V1 simple cells): Useful additions of the last 25 years , 2011, Vision Research.
[35] S. T. Buckland,et al. An Introduction to the Bootstrap. , 1994 .
[36] J A Solomon,et al. Texture interactions determine perceived contrast , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[37] J A Solomon,et al. Perceived pattern regularity computed as a summary statistic: implications for camouflage , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[38] Lewis D. Griffin,et al. Difference magnitude is not measured by discrimination steps for order of point patterns. , 2016, Journal of vision.
[39] F. Kingdom,et al. A common visual metric for approximate number and density , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[40] C. Blakemore,et al. Lateral Inhibition between Orientation Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1970, Nature.
[41] Ewald Hering. Outlines of a theory of the light sense , 1964 .
[42] P. A. van der Helm,et al. The force of symmetry revisited: symmetry-to-noise ratios regulate (a)symmetry effects. , 2004, Acta psychologica.
[43] P. Tiňo,et al. Learning predictive statistics from temporal sequences: Dynamics and strategies , 2017, Journal of vision.
[44] J. Latgé,et al. Correction: Corrigendum: Circulating human basophils lack the features of professional antigen presenting cells , 2013, Scientific Reports.
[45] Marco Bertamini,et al. An Electrophysiological Index of Perceptual Goodness , 2016, Cerebral cortex.
[46] M. Bertamini,et al. Conditions for view invariance in the neural response to visual symmetry. , 2015, Psychophysiology.
[47] Frederick A. A. Kingdom,et al. Introduction and Aims , 2016 .
[48] N. Prins. Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction , 2009 .