Dissociable neural effects of temporal expectations due to passage of time and contextual probability

The human brain is equipped with complex mechanisms to track the changing probability of events in time. While the passage of time itself usually leads to a mounting expectation, context can provide additional information about when events are likely to happen. In this study we dissociate these two sources of temporal expectation in terms of their neural correlates and underlying brain connectivity patterns. We analysed magnetoencephalographic (MEG) data acquired from N=24 healthy participants listening to auditory stimuli. These stimuli could be presented at different temporal intervals but occurred most often at intermediate intervals, forming a contextual probability distribution. Evoked MEG response amplitude was sensitive to both passage of time and contextual probability, albeit at different latencies: the effects of passage of time were observed earlier than the effects of context. The underlying sources of MEG activity were also different across the two types of temporal prediction: the effects of passage of time were localised to early auditory regions and superior temporal gyri, while context was additionally linked to activity in inferior parietal cortices. Finally, these differences were modelled using biophysical (dynamic causal) modelling: passage of time was explained in terms of widespread gain modulation and decreased prediction error signalling at lower levels of the hierarchy, while contextual expectation led to more localised gain modulation and decreased prediction error signalling at higher levels of the hierarchy. These results present a comprehensive account of how independent sources of temporal prediction may be differentially expressed in cortical circuits. HIGHLIGHTS - Predictability of tone onset times affects auditory network connectivity - Foreperiod and distribution of events in time have dissociable neural substrates - Decreased prediction error at different levels of cortical hierarchy

[1]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modeling , 2010, Scholarpedia.

[2]  Liping Wang,et al.  Large-Scale Cortical Networks for Hierarchical Prediction and Prediction Error in the Primate Brain , 2018, Neuron.

[3]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Evidence for a hierarchy of predictions and prediction errors in human cortex , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Jonas Obleser,et al.  Tracking Temporal Hazard in the Human Electroencephalogram Using a Forward Encoding Model , 2017, eNeuro.

[5]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Encoding of Temporal Probabilities in the Human Brain , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Michael W. Spratling Predictive Coding as a Model of Response Properties in Cortical Area V1 , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  F. Vidal,et al.  Functional Anatomy of the Attentional Modulation of Time Estimation , 2004, Science.

[8]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling of precision and synaptic gain in visual perception — an EEG study , 2012, NeuroImage.

[9]  D Akkal,et al.  Time predictability modulates pre-supplementary motor area neuronal activity , 2004, Neuroreport.

[10]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling of lateral interactions in the visual cortex , 2013, NeuroImage.

[11]  Carles Escera,et al.  Repetition suppression and repetition enhancement underlie auditory memory-trace formation in the human brain: an MEG study , 2015, NeuroImage.

[12]  Sylvain Baillet,et al.  Motor origin of temporal predictions in auditory attention , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Repetition suppression and its contextual determinants in predictive coding , 2016, Cortex.

[14]  Rico Fischer,et al.  Priming of visual cortex by temporal attention? The effects of temporal predictability on stimulus(-specific) processing in early visual cortical areas , 2013, NeuroImage.

[15]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Convolution models for induced electromagnetic responses , 2013, NeuroImage.

[16]  Martin Wiener,et al.  Implicit timing activates the left inferior parietal cortex , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  P. Montague,et al.  Ready…Go: Amplitude of the fMRI Signal Encodes Expectation of Cue Arrival Time , 2009, PLoS biology.

[18]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Representation of Time by Neurons in the Posterior Parietal Cortex of the Macaque , 2003, Neuron.

[20]  R. Moran,et al.  Inputs to prefrontal cortex support visual recognition in the aging brain , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[21]  A. Nobre,et al.  Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  F. D. Lange,et al.  How Do Expectations Shape Perception? , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  F. Waszak,et al.  Temporal expectation and spectral expectation operate in distinct fashion on neuronal populations , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Tracking slow modulations in synaptic gain using dynamic causal modelling: Validation in epilepsy , 2015, NeuroImage.

[26]  E. Maris,et al.  Prior Expectation Mediates Neural Adaptation to Repeated Sounds in the Auditory Cortex: An MEG Study , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Tai Sing Lee,et al.  Hierarchical Bayesian inference in the visual cortex. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[28]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting attention in time. Modulation of brain potentials. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A Hierarchy of Time-Scales and the Brain , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[30]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Repetition suppression and plasticity in the human brain , 2009, NeuroImage.

[31]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Multiple sparse priors for the M/EEG inverse problem , 2008, NeuroImage.

[32]  Patrick Berg,et al.  Artifact Correction of the Ongoing EEG Using Spatial Filters Based on Artifact and Brain Signal Topographies , 2002, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[33]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic Causal Models and Physiological Inference: A Validation Study Using Isoflurane Anaesthesia in Rodents , 2011, PloS one.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Attention, Uncertainty, and Free-Energy , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[35]  Jordi Costa-Faidella,et al.  Interactions between “What” and “When” in the Auditory System: Temporal Predictability Enhances Repetition Suppression , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Timing, Storage, and Comparison of Stimulus Duration Engage Discrete Anatomical Components of a Perceptual Timing Network , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Rhythmic gain control during supramodal integration of approximate number , 2016, NeuroImage.

[38]  Ryota Kanai,et al.  Modality-independent role of the primary auditory cortex in time estimation , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  Adeel Razi,et al.  Bayesian model reduction and empirical Bayes for group (DCM) studies , 2016, NeuroImage.

[40]  Caspar M. Schwiedrzik,et al.  Stimulus Predictability Reduces Responses in Primary Visual Cortex , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  Online and offline tools for head movement compensation in MEG , 2013, NeuroImage.

[42]  P. Hagoort,et al.  The suppression of repetition enhancement: A review of fMRI studies , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Geoffrey M. Ghose,et al.  Attentional modulation in visual cortex depends on task timing , 2002, Nature.

[44]  S. Kochen,et al.  Convergent evidence for hierarchical prediction networks from human electrocorticography and magnetoencephalography , 2016, Cortex.

[45]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  When Time Shapes Behavior: fMRI Evidence of Brain Correlates of Temporal Monitoring , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  Timothy H. Muller,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Perceiving the Passage of Time: Neural Possibilities , 2022 .

[47]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[48]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Differential roles for parietal and frontal cortices in fixed versus evolving temporal expectations: Dissociating prior from posterior temporal probabilities with fMRI , 2016, NeuroImage.

[49]  Jan-Mathijs Schoffelen,et al.  Temporal Expectation and Attention Jointly Modulate Auditory Oscillatory Activity in the Beta Band , 2015, PloS one.

[50]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Task relevance modulates the behavioural and neural effects of sensory predictions , 2017, PLoS biology.

[51]  K. Lange The ups and downs of temporal orienting: a review of auditory temporal orienting studies and a model associating the heterogeneous findings on the auditory N1 with opposite effects of attention and prediction , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[52]  Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al.  Less Is More: Expectation Sharpens Representations in the Primary Visual Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[53]  A. Nobre,et al.  The hazards of time , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[54]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling , 2003, NeuroImage.

[55]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Applications of random field theory to electrophysiology , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[56]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Free Energy, Precision and Learning: The Role of Cholinergic Neuromodulation , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.