Increases in rewards promote flexible behavior
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. Jersild. Mental set and shift , 2011 .
[2] Julie D. Golomb,et al. A taxonomy of external and internal attention. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.
[3] Jon Driver,et al. Fortune and reversals of fortune in visual search: Reward contingencies for pop-out targets affect search efficiency and target repetition effects , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[4] Líam Benison. Learning to attend , 2009 .
[5] L. Chelazzi,et al. Learning to Attend and to Ignore Is a Matter of Gains and Losses , 2009, Psychological science.
[6] H. Hughes,et al. On the minimization of task switch costs following long-term training , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[7] S. Kennerley,et al. Reward-Dependent Modulation of Working Memory in Lateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[8] Andrew B. Leber,et al. Neural predictors of moment-to-moment fluctuations in cognitive flexibility , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[9] Wayne D. Gray,et al. An integrated model of cognitive control in task switching. , 2008, Psychological review.
[10] M. Johannesson,et al. Crowding Out in Blood Donation: Was Titmuss Right? , 2008 .
[11] Á. Kristjánsson. "I know what you did on the last trial"--a selective review of research on priming in visual search. , 2008, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.
[12] J Theeuwes,et al. Reward primes visual search , 2008 .
[13] K. Lesch,et al. Dopamine and cognitive control: the prospect of monetary gains influences the balance between flexibility and stability in a set‐shifting paradigm , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.
[14] Marci S. DeCaro,et al. From poor performance to success under stress: working memory, strategy selection, and mathematical problem solving under pressure. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[15] L. Snyder,et al. Extensive practice does not eliminate human switch costs , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[16] L. Pessoa,et al. Motivation sharpens exogenous spatial attention. , 2007, Emotion.
[17] David M Milstein,et al. The Influence of Expected Value on Saccadic Preparation , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[18] Jillian H. Fecteau,et al. Priming of pop-out depends upon the current goals of observers. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[19] A. Tversky,et al. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .
[20] David Badre,et al. Computational and neurobiological mechanisms underlying cognitive flexibility. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[21] L. Chelazzi,et al. Visual Selective Attention and the Effects of Monetary Rewards , 2006, Psychological science.
[22] M. Rabin,et al. A Model of Reference-Dependent Preferences , 2006 .
[23] Tor D Wager,et al. Neuroimaging studies of shifting attention: a meta-analysis , 2004, NeuroImage.
[24] Gordon D Logan,et al. Working memory, task switching, and executive control in the task span procedure. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[25] G. Dreisbach,et al. How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at the cost of increased distractibility. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[26] Gazzaniga,et al. 29 Short-Term Memory for the Rapid Deployment of Visual Attention , 2004 .
[27] Stephen Monsell,et al. The effects of recent practice on task switching. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[28] Jillian H. Fecteau,et al. Exploring the consequences of the previous trial , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[29] G. Logan,et al. Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] R. Kliegl,et al. Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[31] Stephen Monsell,et al. Residual costs in task switching: Testing the failure-to-engage hypothesis , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[32] B. Frey,et al. Motivation crowding theory , 2001 .
[33] D. Meyer,et al. Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al. Attention systems and the organization of the human parietal cortex , 2001, NeuroImage.
[35] R. Hertwig,et al. Experimental practices in economics: A methodological challenge for psychologists? , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[36] J. Driver,et al. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .
[37] A. Rustichini,et al. A Fine is a Price , 2000, The Journal of Legal Studies.
[38] R De Jong,et al. An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .
[39] Colin Camerer,et al. The Effects of Financial Incentives in Experiments: A Review and Capital-Labor-Production Framework , 1999 .
[40] D. Kahneman,et al. Well-being : the foundations of hedonic psychology , 1999 .
[41] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[42] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[43] E. Bizzi,et al. The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .
[44] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[45] Claus Bundesen,et al. Visual selective attention , 1995 .
[46] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[47] M. Moscovitch,et al. Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .
[48] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[49] R. Baumeister. Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. , 1984, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[50] R. Nisbett,et al. Undermining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the "overjustification" hypothesis. , 1973 .
[51] William C. Stebbins. Response latency as a function of amount of reinforcement. , 1962, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[52] James L Olds,et al. Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. , 1954, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[53] B. Skinner,et al. Are theories of learning necessary? , 2013, Psychological review.
[54] W. James. The principles of psychology , 1983 .
[55] G. D. Logan. Task Switching , 2022 .