A Single-Boundary Accumulator Model of Response Times in an Addition Verification Task
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. F. Schouten,et al. Reaction time and accuracy. , 1967, Acta psychologica.
[2] G. Groen,et al. A chronometric analysis of simple addition. , 1972 .
[3] Thomas J. Faulkenberry,et al. Visual Search for Conjunctions of Physical and Numerical Size Shows That They Are Processed Independently , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] Wayne A. Wickelgren,et al. Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing dynamics , 1977 .
[5] W. Schwarz. The ex-Wald distribution as a descriptive model of response times , 2001, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[6] Richard D. Morey,et al. Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .
[7] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. Cognitive arithmetic across cultures. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[8] D. LeBihan,et al. Modulation of Parietal Activation by Semantic Distance in a Number Comparison Task , 2001, NeuroImage.
[9] Roger Ratcliff,et al. The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.
[10] S. Dehaene. Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.
[11] Melissa E. Libertus,et al. Electrophysiological evidence for notation independence in numerical processing , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Functions.
[12] Roger Ratcliff,et al. A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .
[13] R. Ratcliff,et al. Retrieval Processes in Recognition Memory , 1976 .
[14] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. Default Bayes factors for ANOVA designs , 2012 .
[15] Christophe Ley,et al. Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median , 2013 .
[16] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. An encoding-complex approach to numerical cognition in Chinese-English bilinguals. , 2004, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[17] S. Link,et al. A sequential theory of psychological discrimination , 1975 .
[18] R. Duncan Luce,et al. Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .
[19] R. Anders,et al. The shifted Wald distribution for response time data analysis. , 2016, Psychological methods.
[20] Jeffrey Bisanz,et al. Selection of Procedures in Mental Addition: Reassessing the Problem Size Effect in Adults , 1996 .
[21] R Core Team,et al. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .
[22] M. Ashcraft. Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory , 1992, Cognition.
[23] Richard D. Morey,et al. Baysefactor: Computation of Bayes Factors for Common Designs , 2018 .
[24] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. An encoding-complex view of cognitive number processing: Comment on McCloskey, Sokol, and Goodman (1986). , 1988 .
[25] Eric-Jan Wagenmakers,et al. An EZ-diffusion model for response time and accuracy , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[27] Marc Brysbaert,et al. About the influence of the presentation format on arithmetical-fact retrieval processes , 1997, Cognition.
[28] Jamie I. D. Campbell. Architectures for numerical cognition , 1994, Cognition.
[29] Tom Verguts,et al. The size congruity effect: Is bigger always more? , 2011, Cognition.
[30] M. McCloskey. Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia , 1992, Cognition.
[31] Dana Ganor-Stern,et al. Across-notation automatic numerical processing. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[32] Scott D. Brown,et al. Diffusion Decision Model: Current Issues and History , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[33] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[34] Dana Ganor-Stern,et al. Automatic numerical processing is based on an abstract representation , 2009 .
[35] Gordon D. Logan,et al. What everyone finds: The problem-size effect. , 2005 .
[36] M. McCloskey,et al. Chapter 13 The Functional Architecture Of Numerical Processing Mechanisms: Defending The Modular Model , 1992 .
[37] G. Mandler,et al. Retrieval processes in recognition , 1974, Memory & cognition.
[38] J. Andel. Sequential Analysis , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.
[39] Andrew Heathcote,et al. Fitting Wald and ex-Wald distributions to response time data: An example using functions for the S-PLUS package , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[40] Thomas J. Faulkenberry,et al. Response trajectories capture the continuous dynamics of the size congruity effect. , 2016, Acta psychologica.
[41] P. Macizo,et al. Activation and selection of arithmetic facts: The role of numerical format , 2015, Memory & Cognition.
[42] M McCloskey,et al. Representing and using numerical information. , 1995, The American psychologist.
[43] E. Wagenmakers,et al. Psychological interpretation of the ex-Gaussian and shifted Wald parameters: A diffusion model analysis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[44] J Lai,et al. Selection procedures , 2004, British dental journal.
[45] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. Cognitive arithmetic across cultures. , 2001 .
[46] Marcie Penner-Wilger,et al. Calculation latency: The μ of memory and the τ of transformation , 2006 .
[47] Thomas J. Faulkenberry,et al. Bottom-up and top-down attentional contributions to the size congruity effect , 2016, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[48] Jonathan A. Fugelsang,et al. Strategy choice for arithmetic verification: effects of numerical surface form , 2001, Cognition.
[49] L. M. M.-T.. Theory of Probability , 1929, Nature.
[50] R. H. S. Carpenter,et al. Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.