Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object

Three studies investigated infants’ understanding that gaze involves a relation between a person and the object of his or her gaze. Infants were habituated to an event in which an actor turned and looked at one of two toys. Then, infants saw test events in which (1) the actor turned to the same side as during habituation to look at a different toy, or (2) the actor turned to the other side to look at the same toy as during habituation. The first of these involved a change in the relation between actor and object. The second involved a new physical motion on the part of the actor but no change in the relation between actor and object. Seven- and 9-month-old infants did not respond to the change in relation between actor and object, although infants at both ages followed the actor’s gaze to the toys. In contrast, 12-month-old infants responded to the change in the actor‐object relation. Control conditions verified that the paradigm was a sensitive index of the younger infants’ representations of action: 7- and 9-month-olds responded to a change in the actor‐object relation when the actor’s gaze was accompanied by a grasp. Taken together, these findings indicate that gaze-following does not initially go hand in hand with understanding the relation between a person who looks and the object of his or her gaze, and that infants begin to understand this relation between 9 and 12 months.

[1]  M. Haith,et al.  Eye contact and face scanning in early infancy. , 1978, Science.

[2]  Sylvia M. J. Hains,et al.  Look at me: five-month-old infants' sensitivity to very small deviations in eye-gaze during social interactions , 1998 .

[3]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  Infants’ understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action , 2002 .

[4]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Understanding the link between joint attention and language. , 1995 .

[5]  E. Spelke,et al.  Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action. , 1995 .

[6]  D. Muir,et al.  Infant sensitivity to adult eye direction. , 1996, Child development.

[7]  H. Wellman,et al.  Developing intentional understandings , 2001 .

[8]  A. Woodward Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors , 1999 .

[9]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  Is agency skin-deep? Surface attributes influence infants' sensitivity to goal-directed action , 2004 .

[10]  John H. Flavell,et al.  The development of children's knowledge about the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations. , 1988 .

[11]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  An Emergentist Coalition Model for Word Learning , 2000 .

[12]  B. Sodian,et al.  Infants' Understanding of Looking, Pointing, and Reaching as Cues to Goal-Directed Action , 2004 .

[13]  D. Olson,et al.  Developing theories of mind , 1988 .

[14]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Twelve-Month-Old Infants Interpret Action in Context , 2000, Psychological science.

[15]  Daniela K. O'Neill,et al.  Two‐Year‐Old Children's Sensitivity to a Parent's Knowledge State When Making Requests , 1996 .

[16]  J. Bruner,et al.  The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant , 1975, Nature.

[17]  D. Povinelli,et al.  What young chimpanzees know about seeing. , 2000, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[18]  A. Premack,et al.  Causal cognition : a multidisciplinary debate , 1996 .

[19]  V. Bruce,et al.  Reflexive visual orienting in response to the social attention of others , 1999 .

[20]  D. Muir,et al.  A demonstration of gaze following in 3- to 6-month-olds , 1997 .

[21]  G. Butterworth Origins of joint visual attention in infancy. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[22]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Gaze Perception Triggers Reflexive Visuospatial Orienting , 1999 .

[23]  A. Woodward Constraining the Problem Space in Early Word Learning , 2000 .

[24]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[25]  G. Butterworth,et al.  What minds have in common is space : Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy , 1991 .

[26]  Jon Driver,et al.  Adult's Eyes Trigger Shifts of Visual Attention in Human Infants , 1998 .

[27]  B. Repacholi Infants' use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another person's emotional expression. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[28]  H. Rudolph Schaffer,et al.  The child's entry into a social world , 1984 .

[29]  C. Moore,et al.  Joint attention : its origins and role in development , 1995 .

[30]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  How infants make sense of intentional action , 2001 .

[31]  Roberta Michnick Golinkoff,et al.  Becoming a word learner : a debate on lexical acquisition , 2000 .

[32]  D. Maurer,et al.  Developmental changes in the scanning of faces by young infants. , 1976, Child development.

[33]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[34]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. , 1991, Child development.

[35]  C. Moore,et al.  The origins of joint visual attention in infants. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[36]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  The Ontogeny of Social Information Gathering , 1996 .

[37]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Word Learning: A Window on Early Pragmatic Understanding , 1998 .

[38]  P. Bryant,et al.  Young children understanding that looking leads to knowing (so long as they are looking into a single barrel). , 1990, Child development.

[39]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Reflexive Joint Attention Depends on Lateralized Cortical Connections , 2000, Psychological science.

[40]  A. J. Caron,et al.  Infant Perception of the Structural Properties of the Face. , 1973 .

[41]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Gaze detection and the cortical processing of faces: Evidence from infants and adults , 1995 .

[42]  C. Moore,et al.  Social Understanding at the End of the First Year of Life , 1994 .