How language flows when movements don’t: An automated analysis of spontaneous discourse in Parkinson’s disease

To assess the impact of Parkinson's disease (PD) on spontaneous discourse, we conducted computerized analyses of brief monologues produced by 51 patients and 50 controls. We explored differences in semantic fields (via latent semantic analysis), grammatical choices (using part-of-speech tagging), and word-level repetitions (with graph embedding tools). Although overall output was quantitatively similar between groups, patients relied less heavily on action-related concepts and used more subordinate structures. Also, a classification tool operating on grammatical patterns identified monologues as pertaining to patients or controls with 75% accuracy. Finally, while the incidence of dysfluent word repetitions was similar between groups, it allowed inferring the patients' level of motor impairment with 77% accuracy. Our results highlight the relevance of studying naturalistic discourse features to tap the integrity of neural (and, particularly, motor) networks, beyond the possibilities of standard token-level instruments.

[1]  B. Dubois,et al.  Deficit of verb generation in nondemented patients with Parkinson's disease , 2003, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[2]  J. Illes,et al.  Language production in Parkinson's disease: Acoustic and linguistic considerations , 1988, Brain and Language.

[3]  G. Stebbins,et al.  Clinical differences among mild cognitive impairment subtypes in Parkinson's disease , 2012, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[4]  J. Illes Neurolinguistic features of spontaneous language production dissociate three forms of neurodegenerative disease: Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's , 1989, Brain and Language.

[5]  A. Goberman,et al.  Characteristics of speech disfluency in Parkinson disease , 2010, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[6]  P. Svenningsson,et al.  Cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson's disease: diagnosis, biomarkers, and treatment , 2012, The Lancet Neurology.

[7]  A. Hofman,et al.  Prevalence of Parkinson's disease in Europe: A collaborative study of population-based cohorts. Neurologic Diseases in the Elderly Research Group. , 2000, Neurology.

[8]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[9]  Martin Sliwinski,et al.  Clustering strategies on tasks of verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Fernando Cuetos,et al.  Verbal fluency in Parkinson’s disease patients on/off dopamine medication , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  Manuel Menéndez,et al.  Action naming is impaired in Parkinson disease patients , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  M. Ullman Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model , 2004, Cognition.

[13]  L L Murray,et al.  Spoken language production in Huntington's and Parkinson's diseases. , 2000, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[14]  Günther Deuschl,et al.  Acquired stuttering after pallidal deep brain stimulation for dystonia , 2009, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[15]  F. Cuetos,et al.  Action naming in Parkinson's disease patients on/off dopamine , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.

[16]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of cortical function in patients with Parkinson's disease , 2002, Annals of neurology.

[17]  Roberto Eleopra,et al.  To move or not to move: Subthalamic deep brain stimulation effects on implicit motor simulation , 2014, Brain Research.

[18]  Linda Bebout,et al.  Processing of negative morphemes in aphasia: An example of the complexities of the closed class/open class concept , 1993 .

[19]  Rutvik H. Desai,et al.  Parkinson’s disease disrupts both automatic and controlled processing of action verbs , 2013, Brain and Language.

[20]  Agustín Ibáñez,et al.  Two-Person Neuroscience and Naturalistic Social Communication: The Role of Language and Linguistic Variables in Brain-Coupling Research , 2014, Front. Psychiatry.

[21]  W Poewe,et al.  Repetitive speech phenomena in Parkinson's disease , 2000, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[22]  W. Koller Dysfluency (stuttering) in extrapyramidal disease. , 1983, Archives of neurology.

[23]  M. Grossman,et al.  Trying to tell a tale , 2006, Neurology.

[24]  M. Grossman,et al.  Attentional resource and processing speed limitations during sentence processing in Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Brain and Language.

[25]  Byron Bernal,et al.  How Localized are Language Brain Areas? A Review of Brodmann Areas Involvement in Oral Language. , 2016, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[26]  T. Bak,et al.  The neuroscience of action semantics in neurodegenerative brain diseases. , 2013, Current opinion in neurology.

[27]  Ramon Ferrer i Cancho,et al.  The small world of human language , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[28]  P. McNamara,et al.  Grammaticality judgments and sentence comprehension in Parkinson's disease: a comparison with Broca's aphasia. , 1996, The International journal of neuroscience.

[29]  Jesús Francisco Vargas-Bonilla,et al.  Towards an automatic monitoring of the neurological state of Parkinson's patients from speech , 2016, 2016 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP).

[30]  A. Lees,et al.  Ageing and Parkinson's disease: substantia nigra regional selectivity. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  L Lopiano,et al.  Sexual well being in parkinsonian patients after deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus , 2004, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[32]  J. Hughes,et al.  Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. , 1992, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[33]  S. Kotz,et al.  Non-motor basal ganglia functions: A review and proposal for a model of sensory predictability in auditory language perception , 2009, Cortex.

[34]  G. Lakoff,et al.  The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[35]  Luke E. Miller,et al.  An investigation of semantic similarity judgments about action and non-action verbs in Parkinson's disease: implications for the Embodied Cognition Framework , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[36]  J. Jankovic,et al.  Movement Disorder Society‐sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS‐UPDRS): Scale presentation and clinimetric testing results , 2008, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[37]  Philip Lieberman,et al.  Speech production, syntax comprehension, and cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease , 1992, Brain and Language.

[38]  Harriet de Wit,et al.  A Window into the Intoxicated Mind? Speech as an Index of Psychoactive Drug Effects , 2014, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[39]  J R Orozco-Arroyave,et al.  Automatic detection of Parkinson's disease in running speech spoken in three different languages. , 2016, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[40]  M. Sigman,et al.  Automated analysis of free speech predicts psychosis onset in high-risk youths , 2015, npj Schizophrenia.

[41]  M. Murer,et al.  Levodopa in Parkinson’s Disease , 1999, Drug safety.

[42]  V. Sturm,et al.  Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus reversibly deteriorates stuttering in advanced Parkinson’s disease , 2006, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[43]  Heather Harris Wright,et al.  Lexical diversity for adults with and without aphasia across discourse elicitation tasks , 2011, Aphasiology.

[44]  E. Dietrichs,et al.  Aggravated stuttering following subthalamic deep brain stimulation in Parkinson's disease - two cases , 2011, BMC neurology.

[45]  Peter Hagoort,et al.  MUC (Memory, Unification, Control) and beyond , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[46]  Michael T. Ullman,et al.  Is Broca's Area Part of a Basal Ganglia Thalamocortical Circuit? , 2006, Cortex.

[47]  P. Hagoort Nodes and networks in the neural architecture for language: Broca's region and beyond , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[48]  M. Albert,et al.  Speech monitoring skills in Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, and normal aging , 1992, Brain and Language.

[49]  Hiroko Nakano,et al.  The roles of sequencing and verbal working memory in sentence comprehension deficits in Parkinson’s disease , 2006, Brain and Language.

[50]  Murray Grossman,et al.  Assessing Resource Demands during Sentence Processing in Parkinson's Disease , 2002, Brain and Language.

[51]  S. Leder,et al.  Adult onset of stuttering as a presenting sign in a parkinsonian-like syndrome: a case report. , 1996, Journal of communication disorders.

[52]  Y. Lebrun Cluttering after brain damage , 1996 .

[53]  David A Copland,et al.  The influence of dopamine on semantic activation in Parkinson's disease: evidence from a multipriming task. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[54]  S. Fahn,et al.  Speech Dysfluency Exacerbated by Levodopa in Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[55]  P. Stanzione,et al.  Grammar improvement following deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic and the pedunculopontine nuclei in advanced Parkinson's disease: a pilot study. , 2009, Parkinsonism & related disorders.

[56]  O. Kinouchi,et al.  Speech Graphs Provide a Quantitative Measure of Thought Disorder in Psychosis , 2012, PloS one.

[57]  M. Pell,et al.  Effects of verbal working memory deficits on metaphor comprehension in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2007, Brain and Language.

[58]  Dean C. Delis,et al.  Visual selective attention deficits in patients with Parkinson's disease: A quantitative model-based approach. , 1996 .

[59]  P. McNamara,et al.  Pragmatic comprehension deficit in Parkinson's disease , 2010, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[60]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Word processing in Parkinson's disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  J Rispens,et al.  Negation in agrammatism: a cross-linguistic comparison , 2001, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[62]  J. Nutt,et al.  Parkinson's disease , 2004, The Lancet.

[63]  G. Fénelon,et al.  Improvement in developmental stuttering following deep brain stimulation for Parkinson's disease. , 2013, Parkinsonism & related disorders.

[64]  T. Landauer,et al.  Indexing by Latent Semantic Analysis , 1990 .

[65]  Agustín Ibáñez,et al.  A touch with words: Dynamic synergies between manual actions and language , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[66]  Laura L. Murray,et al.  Productive syntax abilities in Huntington's and Parkinson's diseases , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[67]  Jesús Francisco Vargas-Bonilla,et al.  New Spanish speech corpus database for the analysis of people suffering from Parkinson’s disease , 2014, LREC.

[68]  Murray Grossman,et al.  Sentence comprehension in Parkinson's disease: The role of attention and memory , 1992, Brain and Language.

[69]  Mariano Sigman,et al.  Global organization of the Wordnet lexicon , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[70]  R. Grace,et al.  Characteristics of executive function impairment in Parkinson’s disease patients without dementia , 2009, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[71]  B. Schmand,et al.  Cognitive profile of patients with newly diagnosed Parkinson disease , 2005, Neurology.

[72]  A. Ibáñez,et al.  Words in motion: Motor-language coupling in Parkinson’s disease , 2014 .

[73]  B. Crosson,et al.  Narrative Discourse Cohesion in Early Stage Parkinson's Disease. , 2015, Journal of Parkinson's disease.

[74]  A. Ibanez,et al.  Motor-language coupling: Direct evidence from early Parkinson’s disease and intracranial cortical recordings , 2013, Cortex.

[75]  F. Manes,et al.  Action-verb processing in Parkinson’s disease: new pathways for motor–language coupling , 2013, Brain Structure and Function.

[76]  J. Playfer,et al.  Verbal fluency in parkinson's disease , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[77]  Patrick McNamara,et al.  Pragmatic communication skills in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Brain and Language.

[78]  Sidarta Ribeiro,et al.  Graph analysis of dream reports is especially informative about psychosis , 2014, Scientific Reports.

[79]  W. Oertel,et al.  Palilalia as a symptom of levodopa induced hyperkinesia in Parkinson's disease. , 1989, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[80]  Edward G. Conture,et al.  Stuttering: Its Nature, Diagnosis and Treatment , 2000 .

[81]  Agustín Ibáñez,et al.  Cortical dynamics and subcortical signatures of motor-language coupling in Parkinson’s disease , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[82]  P. Santens,et al.  Quantitative analysis of language production in Parkinson's disease using a cued sentence generation task , 2012, Clinical linguistics & phonetics.

[83]  A. Ibáñez,et al.  Syntax, action verbs, action semantics, and object semantics in Parkinson's disease: Dissociability, progression, and executive influences , 2015, Cortex.

[84]  E. Bézard,et al.  Initial clinical manifestations of Parkinson's disease: features and pathophysiological mechanisms , 2009, The Lancet Neurology.

[85]  W. Poewe,et al.  Movement Disorder Society Task Force report on the Hoehn and Yahr staging scale: Status and recommendations The Movement Disorder Society Task Force on rating scales for Parkinson's disease , 2004, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[86]  Michael Halliday,et al.  An Introduction to Functional Grammar , 1985 .

[87]  Michael Blomgren,et al.  Parkinsonian speech disfluencies: effects of L-dopa-related fluctuations. , 2003, Journal of fluency disorders.