Modularity, abstractness and the interactive brain
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. , 1982, Psychological review.
[2] E. Haan,et al. Behavioural and Physiological Evidence for Covert Face Recognition in a Prosopagnosic Patient , 1992, Cortex.
[3] N. Chater,et al. Against Logicist Cognitive Science , 1991 .
[4] J. Driver,et al. Can Visual Neglect Operate in Object-centred Co-ordinates? An Affirmative Single-case Study , 1991 .
[5] D. Homa,et al. Perceptibility of schematic face stimuli: Evidence for a perceptual Gestalt , 1976, Memory & cognition.
[6] J. H. Jacksons. On affections of speech from disease of the brain , 1878 .
[7] R A Johnston,et al. Understanding face recognition with an interactive activation model. , 1990, British journal of psychology.
[8] A. Caramazza,et al. Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia , 1976, Brain and Language.
[9] A. Young,et al. Implicit access to semantic information , 1989, Brain and Cognition.
[10] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Independent hemispheric attentional systems mediate visual search in split-brain patients , 1989, Nature.
[11] T. Shallice,et al. Perseverative and Semantic Influences on Visual Object Naming Errors in Optic Aphasia: A Connectionist Account , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[12] N. Chater,et al. Autonomy, implementation and cognitive architecture: A reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn , 1990, Cognition.
[13] D. Massaro. Some criticisms of connectionist models of human performance , 1988 .
[14] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. A verbal-semantic category-specific recognition impairment , 1993 .
[15] Ja Bullinaria,et al. DOUBLE DISSOCIATION IN ARTIFICIAL NEURAL NETWORKS - IMPLICATIONS FOR NEUROPSYCHOLOGY , 1993 .
[16] N. Geschwind,et al. Mechanisms of Change after Brain Lesions a , 1985, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[17] David LaBerge,et al. Thalamic and Cortical Mechanisms of Attention Suggested by Recent Positron Emission Tomographic Experiments , 1990, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[18] R. Berndt,et al. Category-specific naming deficit following cerebral infarction , 1985, Nature.
[19] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. An encoding-complex view of cognitive number processing: Comment on McCloskey, Sokol, and Goodman (1986). , 1988 .
[20] Nicholas Port,et al. A Neural Network Model of Cortical Activity during Reaching , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[21] K. Kirsner,et al. Discovering functionally independent mental processes: the principle of reversed association. , 1988, Psychological review.
[22] Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al. Integrated versus modular theories of number skills and acalculia , 1991, Brain and Cognition.
[23] V. Mountcastle,et al. Parietal lobe mechanisms for directed visual attention. , 1977, Journal of neurophysiology.
[24] Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al. How neural networks learn from experience. , 1992, Scientific American.
[25] E K Warrington,et al. Prosopagnosia: A Reclassification , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[26] D. J. Felleman,et al. Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.
[27] D. Dennett,et al. The Nature of Consciousness , 2006 .
[28] G. Miller,et al. Semantic networks of english , 1991, Cognition.
[29] A. Caramazza. On drawing inferences about the structure of normal cognitive systems from the analysis of patterns of impaired performance: The case for single-patient studies , 1986, Brain and Cognition.
[30] R. Tegnér,et al. Through a looking glass. A new technique to demonstrate directional hypokinesia in unilateral neglect. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[31] L. Jakobson,et al. A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.
[32] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] D. Bishop,et al. The underlying nature of specific language impairment. , 1992, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.
[34] Jules Davidoff,et al. Recognition of unfamiliar faces in prosopagnosia , 1990, Neuropsychologia.
[35] D. Howard,et al. Object naming in aphasics—the lack of effect of context or realism , 1977, Neuropsychologia.
[36] L. Squire. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. , 1992, Psychological review.
[37] John A. Bullinaria,et al. Neural network models of reading multi-syllabic words , 1993, Proceedings of 1993 International Conference on Neural Networks (IJCNN-93-Nagoya, Japan).
[38] N J Cohen,et al. Cognitive representations and processes in arithmetic: inferences from the performance of brain-damaged subjects. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[39] G. Humphreys,et al. Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[40] A. Young,et al. Understanding covert recognition , 1991, Cognition.
[41] Stephen Grossberg,et al. Competitive Learning: From Interactive Activation to Adaptive Resonance , 1987, Cogn. Sci..
[42] E. Warrington. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology the Selective Impairment of Semantic Memory the Selective Impairment of Semantic Memory , 2022 .
[43] M. Corbetta,et al. A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[44] G. Reeke,et al. Behaviorally based modeling and computational approaches to neuroscience. , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.
[45] P. Goldman-Rakic,et al. Preface: Cerebral Cortex Has Come of Age , 1991 .
[46] Stephen Crain,et al. Language mechanisms and reading disorder: A modular approach , 1986, Cognition.
[47] S. Kosslyn,et al. Components of high-level vision: A cognitive neuroscience analysis and accounts of neurological syndromes , 1990, Cognition.
[48] Yuri Kanin,et al. Soviet Academy faces grass-roots revolt , 1989, Nature.
[49] E. Warrington,et al. CATEGORY SPECIFIC ACCESS DYSPHASIA , 1983 .
[50] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[51] Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al. Lesioning an attractor network: investigations of acquired dyslexia , 1991 .
[52] M. Mozer,et al. On the Interaction of Selective Attention and Lexical Knowledge: A Connectionist Account of Neglect Dyslexia , 1990, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[53] J. Jackson,et al. ON THE ANATOMICAL & PHYSIOLOGICAL LOCALISATION OF MOVEMENTS IN THE BRAIN. , 1873 .
[54] C. Guariglia,et al. Personal and extrapersonal space: A case of neglect dissociation , 1992, Neuropsychologia.
[55] Bruno Debruille,et al. Brain potentials reveal covert facial recognition in prosopagnosia , 1989, Neuropsychologia.
[56] D. Swinney,et al. The Effects of Focal Brain Damage on Sentence Processing: an examination of the neurological organization of a mental module , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[57] J. Fodor. The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .
[58] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Lesioning a connectionist model of visual search: selective effects on distractor grouping. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.
[59] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Computational Consequences of a Bias toward Short Connections , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[60] Elizabeth K. Warrington,et al. Neuropsychological studies of verbal semantic systems , 1981 .
[61] Jerome A. Feldman,et al. Connectionist Models and Their Properties , 1982, Cogn. Sci..
[62] T. Shallice,et al. Category specific semantic impairments , 1984 .
[63] Gerald S. Wasserman. Isomorphism, task dependence, and the multiple meaning theory of neural coding. , 1992 .
[64] F. J. Friedrich,et al. Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[65] A. Young,et al. Face recognition without awareness , 1987 .
[66] M. M. Meyer,et al. Can recognotion of living things be selectively impaired? , 1991, Neuropsychologia.
[67] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Spatial representation of words in the brain implied by studies of a unilateral neglect patient , 1990, Nature.
[68] M. Schwartz,et al. DISSOCIATIONS OF LANGUAGE IN APHASIA: IMPLICATIONS FOR NORMAL FUNCTION * , 1976, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[69] S. Auerbach,et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of attention and memory disorders in traumatic brain injury: an application of neurobehavioral subtypes , 1986 .
[70] Ianthi Maria Tsimpli,et al. Linguistic modularity? A case study of a 'Savant' linguist* , 1991 .
[71] J. Hughlingsjackson,et al. ON AFFECTIONS OF SPEECH FROM DISEASE OF THE BRAIN. , 1879 .
[72] J. Marshall,et al. Syntactic and semantic errors in paralexia , 1966 .
[73] Randall C. O'Reilly,et al. Dissociated overt and covert recognition as an emergent property of a lesioned neural network. , 1993, Psychological review.
[74] John H. R. Maunsell,et al. How parallel are the primate visual pathways? , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.
[75] C. Eriksen,et al. The unconscious perception of the meaning of verbal stimuli. , 1960, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.
[76] Y. Grodzinsky. Language deficits and the theory of syntax , 1986, Brain and Language.
[77] A. Young,et al. Faces Interfere with Name Classification in a Prosopagnosic Patient , 1987, Cortex.
[78] Kenneth M. Heilman,et al. Hemispatial neglect affected by non‐neglected stimuli , 1988, Neurology.
[79] M. Farah,et al. A unified account of cognitive impairments following frontal lobe damage: the role of working memory in complex, organized behavior. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[80] M. Behrmann,et al. Surface dyslexia and dysgraphia: dual routes, single lexicon , 1992 .
[81] George Mandler,et al. Nonspecific Effects of Exposure on Stimuli That Cannot Be Recognized , 1987 .
[82] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. The logic of neuropsychological research and the problem of patient classification in aphasia , 1984, Brain and Language.
[83] C. R. Flowers,et al. Aphasic adults' use of heuristic and structural linguistic cues for sentence analysis , 1982, Brain and Language.
[84] M. Silveri,et al. Interaction between vision and language in category-specific semantic impairment , 1988 .
[85] V Bruce,et al. Modelling face recognition. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[86] E. Saffran,et al. Neuropsychological approaches to the study of language. , 1982, British journal of psychology.
[87] M. Goldberg,et al. Visuospatial and motor attention in the monkey , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[88] E. Warrington,et al. Categories of knowledge. Further fractionations and an attempted integration. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[89] Tim Shallice,et al. Lexical processing in the absence of explicit word identification: Evidence from a letter-by-letter reader , 1986 .
[90] David Caplan,et al. Assignment of thematic roles to nouns in sentence comprehension by an agrammatic patient , 1986, Brain and Language.
[91] Mark A. Fanty,et al. Computing with structured neural networks , 1988, Computer.
[92] Zenon W. Pylyshyn,et al. Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis , 1988, Cognition.
[93] James L. McClelland,et al. A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.
[94] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Cognitive neuropsychology and language: The state of the art , 1988 .
[95] E. Warrington,et al. Phonological Reading: Phenomena and Paradoxes , 1986, Cortex.
[96] John R. Anderson. Arguments concerning representations for mental imagery. , 1978 .
[97] J. Marshall,et al. Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.
[98] Francis Crick,et al. The recent excitement about neural networks , 1989, Nature.
[99] M M Mesulam,et al. Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.
[100] A. Caramazza,et al. Category-specific naming and comprehension impairment: a double dissociation. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[101] James L. McClelland,et al. Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .
[102] Michael I. Jordan,et al. A more biologically plausible learning rule than backpropagation applied to a network model of cortical area 7a. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.
[103] Steven Pinker,et al. Lexical and Conceptual Semantics , 1991 .
[104] Zurif Eb,et al. Language mechanisms: a neuropsychological perspective. , 1980 .
[105] R. Zajonc,et al. Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. , 1980, Science.
[106] M. McCloskey,et al. In defense of a modular architecture for the number-processing system: reply to Campbell and Clark. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[107] M. Young,et al. Sparse population coding of faces in the inferotemporal cortex. , 1992, Science.
[108] M. McCloskey. Networks and Theories: The Place of Connectionism in Cognitive Science , 1991 .
[109] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. The multiple semantics hypothesis: Multiple confusions? , 1990 .
[110] Giuseppe Sartori,et al. The oyster with four legs: A neuropsychological study on the interaction of visual and semantic information , 1998 .
[111] Raymond Bruyer,et al. Heterogeneity in the cognitive manifestations of prosopagnosia: The study of a group of single cases , 1993 .
[112] D. Teller,et al. Sensitization by annular surrounds: sensitization and dark adaptation. , 1969, Vision research.
[113] John C. Marshall,et al. Left Visuo-Spatial Neglect: A Meaningless Entity? , 1992, Cortex.
[114] H. Kelley. Common-Sense Psychology and Scientific Psychology , 1992 .
[115] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[116] A Caramazza,et al. Is Cognitive Neuropsychology Possible? , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[117] James L. McClelland,et al. A computational model of semantic memory impairment: modality specificity and emergent category specificity. , 1991 .
[118] L. Jakobson,et al. A kinematic analysis of reaching and grasping movements in a patient recovering from optic ataxia , 1991, Neuropsychologia.
[119] M. Kinsbourne,et al. A model for the mechanism of unilateral neglect of space. , 1970, Transactions of the American Neurological Association.
[120] J. Baron,et al. PET studies of cortical diaschisis in patients with motor hemi-neglect , 1991, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.
[121] T. R. Jordan,et al. Perception and action in 'visual form agnosia'. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[122] M. McCloskey. Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia , 1992, Cognition.
[123] James L. McClelland,et al. Levels indeed! A response to Broadbent , 1985 .
[124] Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al. NETtalk: a parallel network that learns to read aloud , 1988 .
[125] J. Henderson,et al. The spatial distribution of attention following an exogenous cue , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[126] Kenneth M Heilman,et al. Impaired shifting of attention in Balint's syndrome , 1990, Brain and Cognition.
[127] David Caplan,et al. On the cerebral localization of linguistic functions: Logical and empirical issues surrounding deficit analysis and functional localization , 1981, Brain and Language.
[128] A R Damasio,et al. The limbic system and the localisation of herpes simplex encephalitis. , 1985, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[129] Roy Eagleson,et al. Connectionist networks do not model brain function , 1992 .
[130] A. Young,et al. Understanding face recognition. , 1986, British journal of psychology.
[131] J C Morris,et al. Progressive aphasia , 1990, Neurology.
[132] E Capitani,et al. Progressive language impairment without dementia: a case with isolated category specific semantic defect. , 1988, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[133] M. McCloskey,et al. Cognitive processes in verbal-number production: inferences from the performance of brain-damaged subjects. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[134] M. Mesulam. A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.
[135] C. Geula,et al. Architecture of connectivity within a cingulo-fronto-parietal neurocognitive network for directed attention. , 1993, Archives of neurology.
[136] R Tissot,et al. Qualitative analysis of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to laterality of cerebral lesions , 1972, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[137] D. Sparks,et al. Population coding of the direction, amplitude, and velocity of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.
[138] R. Bauer,et al. Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: A neuropsychological application of the guilty knowledge test , 1984, Neuropsychologia.
[139] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Cognitive mechanisms in number processing and calculation: Evidence from dyscalculia , 1985, Brain and Cognition.
[140] M. Posner,et al. How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[141] R. Nebes. Semantic memory in Alzheimer's disease. , 1989, Psychological bulletin.
[142] Martha J. Farah,et al. Cognitive Neuropsychology: Patterns of Co-occurrence Among the Associative Agnosias: Implications for Visual Object Representation , 1991 .
[143] M. Mesulam,et al. Dissociated neglect behavior following sequential strokes in the right hemisphere , 1990, Annals of neurology.
[144] Steven Pinker,et al. Introduction to special issue of Cognition on lexical and conceptual semantics , 1991, Cognition.
[145] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .
[146] D. Swinney. Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects , 1979 .
[147] Tim Shallice,et al. Précis of From neuropsychology to mental structure , 1991, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[148] T. Shallice,et al. Deep Dyslexia: A Case Study of , 1993 .
[149] T Shallice,et al. The selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory. , 1969, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[150] Michael I. Jordan,et al. A more biologically plausible learning rule for neural networks. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[151] D. Swinney,et al. An On-Line Analysis of Syntactic Processing in Broca′s and Wernicke′s Aphasia , 1993, Brain and Language.
[152] V. Pietrini,et al. Recovery from herpes simplex encephalitis: selective impairment of specific semantic categories with neuroradiological correlation. , 1988, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[153] Nick Chater,et al. A phonologically motivated input representation for the modelling of auditory word perception in continuous speech , 1992 .
[154] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Neglect dyslexia for numbers? a case report , 1991 .
[155] D Swinney,et al. Real-time examinations of lexical processing in aphasics , 1991, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[156] G. Humphreys,et al. Visual object processing in optic aphasia: a case of semantic access agnosia , 1987 .
[157] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. The basis of consistency effects in word naming , 1990 .
[158] M. Perenin,et al. Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. I. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[159] John Hart,et al. Neural subsystems for object knowledge , 1992, Nature.
[160] James L. McClelland. Toward a theory of information processing in graded, random, and interactive networks , 1993 .
[161] M. F. Garrett,et al. Objects of psycholinguistic enquiry , 1981, Cognition.