Simplifying Reading: Applying the Simplicity Principle to Reading

Debates concerning the types of representations that aid reading acquisition have often been influenced by the relationship between measures of early phonological awareness (the ability to process speech sounds) and later reading ability. Here, a complementary approach is explored, analyzing how the functional utility of different representational units, such as whole words, bodies (letters representing the vowel and final consonants of a syllable), and graphemes (letters representing a phoneme) may change as the number of words that can be read gradually increases. Utility is measured by applying a Simplicity Principle to the problem of mapping from print to sound; that is, assuming that the "best" representational units for reading are those which allow the mapping from print to sounds to be encoded as efficiently as possible. Results indicate that when only a small number of words are read whole-word representations are most useful, whereas when many words can be read graphemic representations have the highest utility.

[1]  Jamie L. Metsala,et al.  Word Recognition in Beginning Literacy , 2013 .

[2]  Jason Riggle,et al.  Information theoretic approaches to phonological structure: the case of Finnish vowel harmony , 2012 .

[3]  K. Spencer Predicting children's word-reading accuracy for common English words: the effect of word transparency and complexity. , 2010, British journal of psychology.

[4]  Janet I. Vousden Comparing the content of English reading programs using the Simplicity Principle , 2010 .

[5]  D. Plaut,et al.  SD-squared revisited: reply to Coltheart, Tree, and Saunders (2010). , 2010, Psychological review.

[6]  Philip C. Abrami,et al.  A Randomized Controlled Trial Study of the ABRACADABRA Reading Intervention Program in Grade 1. , 2009 .

[7]  J. Vousden,et al.  Real books vs reading schemes: a new perspective from instructional psychology , 2009 .

[8]  R. Treiman,et al.  Learning to label letters by sounds or names: a comparison of England and the United States. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[9]  Kenneth A. Spencer,et al.  Feedforward, -backward, and neutral transparency measures for British English , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[10]  Jonathan Solity,et al.  Delivering phonological and phonics training within whole-class teaching. , 2008, The British journal of educational psychology.

[11]  Dominic Wyse,et al.  Synthetic phonics and the teaching of reading , 2008 .

[12]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  Are Young Children Logographic Readers and Spellers? , 2008 .

[13]  Melanie R. Kuhn,et al.  The influence of reading unit size on the development of Stroop interference in early word decoding , 2008, Reading and writing.

[14]  J. Vousden Units of English spelling‐to‐sound mapping: a rational approach to reading instruction , 2008 .

[15]  L. Shapiro,et al.  Developing the practice of educational psychologists through theory and research , 2008, Educational and Child Psychology.

[16]  J. Goldsmith Probability for linguists , 2007 .

[17]  D. Pelli,et al.  Parts, Wholes, and Context in Reading: A Triple Dissociation , 2007, PloS one.

[18]  Dorothy V. M. Bishop,et al.  Vocabulary Is Important for Some, but Not All Reading Skills , 2007 .

[19]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP+ model of reading aloud. , 2007, Psychological review.

[20]  D. Plaut,et al.  Does the PMSP connectionist model of single word reading learn to read in the same way as a child , 2006 .

[21]  Janice F. Almasi,et al.  Comprehen-sion and the Report of the National Reading Panel , 2006 .

[22]  Sang Joon Kim,et al.  A Mathematical Theory of Communication , 2006 .

[23]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  Influence of consonantal context on the reading of vowels: evidence from children. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[24]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Poverty of the Stimulus? A Rational Approach , 2006 .

[25]  Judith A. Bowey,et al.  The Efficacy of Orthographic Rime, Grapheme–Phoneme Correspondence, and Implicit Phonics Approaches to Teaching Decoding Skills , 2005 .

[26]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  The differential role of phonological and distributional cues in grammatical categorisation , 2005, Cognition.

[27]  Frauke M. Hellwig,et al.  Onset Entropy Matters – Letter-to-phoneme Mappings in Seven Languages , 2005 .

[28]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[29]  Nick Chater,et al.  The differential contribution of phonological and distributional cues in grammatical categorisation. , 2005 .

[30]  In Jae Myung,et al.  A minimum description length principle for perception , 2005 .

[31]  Nick Chater,et al.  What can be learned from positive data? Insights from an ‘ideal learner’. Commentary on ‘A Multiple process solution to the logical problem of language acquisition’ by Brian MacWhinney , 2004, Journal of Child Language.

[32]  Jackie Masterson,et al.  How long do the advantages of learning to read a transparent orthography last? An investigation of the reading skills and reading impairment of Welsh children at 10 years of age , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[33]  R. Treiman,et al.  Pronouncing novel graphemes: The role of consonantal context , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[34]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Do current connectionist learning models account for reading development in different languages? , 2004, Cognition.

[35]  N. Chater,et al.  Acquisition and Evolution of Natural Language Acquisition and Evolution of quasiregular languages : Two puzzles for the price of one , 2004 .

[36]  Morag Stuart,et al.  Children's early reading vocabulary: description and word frequency lists. , 2003, The British journal of educational psychology.

[37]  Charles Hulme,et al.  The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[38]  P. H. Seymour,et al.  Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. , 2003, British journal of psychology.

[39]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  Influence of consonantal context on the pronunciation of vowels: A comparison of human readers and computational models , 2003, Cognition.

[40]  P F D Gontijo,et al.  Grapheme—phoneme probabilities in British English , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[41]  N. Chater,et al.  Similarity as transformation , 2003, Cognition.

[42]  N. Chater,et al.  Simplicity: a unifying principle in cognitive science? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  Peter J. Hatcher,et al.  Phoneme awareness is a better predictor of early reading skill than onset-rime awareness. , 2002, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[44]  J. Masterson,et al.  The effects of familiarity, orthographic neighbourhood density, letter-length and graphemic complexity on children's reading accuracy. , 2002, British Journal of Psychology.

[45]  Nick Chater,et al.  A simplicity principle in unsupervised human categorization , 2002, Cogn. Sci..

[46]  John Goldsmith,et al.  Probabilistic Models of Grammar: Phonology as Information Minimization , 2002 .

[47]  Nick C. Ellis,et al.  Why learning to read is easier in Welsh than in English: Orthographic transparency effects evinced with frequency-matched tests , 2001, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[48]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTEREST HOW PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE INFORMS THE TEACHING OF READING , 2022 .

[49]  Simon Kirby,et al.  The Survival of the Smallest: Stability Conditions for the Cultural Evolution of Compositional Language , 2001, ECAL.

[50]  Steven A. Stahl,et al.  Systematic Phonics Instruction Helps Students Learn to Read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s Meta-Analysis , 2001 .

[51]  John A. Goldsmith,et al.  Unsupervised Learning of the Morphology of a Natural Language , 2001, CL.

[52]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  Relationships between sounds and letters in English monosyllables , 2001 .

[53]  D. Bishop Genetic influences on language impairment and literacy problems in children: same or different? , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[54]  C. E. SHANNON,et al.  A mathematical theory of communication , 1948, MOCO.

[55]  A Small-to-Large Unit Progression in Metaphonological Awareness and Reading? , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[56]  Jacob Feldman,et al.  Minimization of Boolean complexity in human concept learning , 2000, Nature.

[57]  Karin Landerl,et al.  Influences of orthographic consistency and reading instruction on the development of nonword reading skills , 2000 .

[58]  J. Masterson,et al.  Spongelike acquisition of sight vocabulary in beginning readers , 2000 .

[59]  D. Langenberg Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction , 2000 .

[60]  Mike Dowman,et al.  Addressing the Learnability of Verb Subcategorization with Bayesian Inference , 2000 .

[61]  M. Stuart Getting ready for reading: Early phoneme awareness and phonics teaching improves reading and spelling in inner‐city second language learners , 1999 .

[62]  G. Brown,et al.  Units of analysis in nonword reading: evidence from children and adults. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[63]  D. Bourassa,et al.  Fast and slow namers: benefits of segmentation and whole word training. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[64]  N. Chater The Search for Simplicity: A Fundamental Cognitive Principle? , 1999 .

[65]  J. Masterson,et al.  Inferring Sublexical Correspondences from Sight Vocabulary: Evidence from 6- and 7-year-olds , 1999, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[66]  N. Chater,et al.  Rational models of cognition , 1998 .

[67]  P Bryant Sensitivity to onset and rhyme does predict young Children's reading: a comment on Muter, Hulme, Snowling, and Taylor (1997) , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[68]  Muter,et al.  Segmentation does predict early progress in learning to read better than rhyme: A reply to bryant , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[69]  Sally Andrews,et al.  Rule and analogy mechanisms in reading nonwords: Hough dou peapel rede gnew wirds? , 1998 .

[70]  Savage,et al.  Sublexical Inferences in Beginning Reading: Medial Vowel Digraphs as Functional Units of Transfer. , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[71]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Learning to Segment Speech Using Multiple Cues: A Connectionist Model , 1998 .

[72]  L. Siegel,et al.  Reading and spelling acquisition in French: the role of phonological mediation and orthographic factors. , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[73]  U. Goswami,et al.  Children's orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French, and Spanish , 1998, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[74]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .

[75]  Linnea C. Ehri,et al.  Grapheme-phoneme knowledge is essential for learning to read words in English , 1998 .

[76]  Uta Frith,et al.  Differences in Phonological Recoding in German- and English-Speaking Children. , 1998 .

[77]  J. Metsala,et al.  Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: precursors to p , 1998 .

[78]  Keith T. Greaney,et al.  Effects of rime-based orthographic analogy training on the word recognition skills of children with reading disability. , 1997 .

[79]  Nick Chater,et al.  Simplicity and the mind , 1997 .

[80]  R. Treiman,et al.  Syllable Structure and the Distribution of Phonemes in English Syllables , 1997 .

[81]  Betty Ann Levy,et al.  Beginning Word Recognition: Benefits of Training by Segmentation and Whole Word Methods , 1997 .

[82]  C. Hulme,et al.  Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[83]  Charles Hulme,et al.  Phonemic Segmentation, Not Onset-Rime Segmentation, Predicts Early Reading and Spelling Skills. , 1997 .

[84]  R S Savage Do children need concurrent prompts in order to use lexical analogies in reading? , 1997, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[85]  T. A. Cartwright,et al.  Distributional regularity and phonotactic constraints are useful for segmentation , 1996, Cognition.

[86]  Nick Chater,et al.  Reconciling simplicity and likelihood principles in perceptual organization. , 1996, Psychological review.

[87]  R. H. Baayen,et al.  The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .

[88]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[89]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[90]  D. Share Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition , 1995, Cognition.

[91]  J. Marshall,et al.  Acquisition of reading and written spelling in a transparent orthography: Two non parallel processes? , 1995 .

[92]  C. Hulme,et al.  The Role of Phonology in Young Children Learning to Read Words: The Direct-Mapping Hypothesis. , 1994 .

[93]  V. Muter,et al.  Orthographic analogies and phonological awareness: their role and significance in early reading development. , 1994, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[94]  H. Wimmer,et al.  The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: word recognition in English and German children , 1994, Cognition.

[95]  M. Adams Reading Acquisition. , 1993 .

[96]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[97]  U. Goswami,et al.  Phonological Skills and Learning to Read , 1993, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[98]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system , 1993, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[99]  Ming Li,et al.  An Introduction to Kolmogorov Complexity and Its Applications , 2019, Texts in Computer Science.

[100]  Barbara R. Foorman,et al.  Effects of Three Orthographic/Phonological Units on First-Grade Reading , 1992 .

[101]  Rebecca Treiman,et al.  Learning to Pronounce Words: The Limitations of Analogies. , 1992 .

[102]  Linnea C. Ehri,et al.  Reconceptualizing the Development of Sight Word Reading and Its Relationship to Recoding , 1992 .

[103]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Reflections of the Environment in Memory Form of the Memory Functions , 2022 .

[104]  I. S. Brown,et al.  Effects of instruction on beginning reading skills in children at risk for reading disability , 1990 .

[105]  C. D. Porpodas,et al.  Phonological and lexical encoding processes in beginning readers: Effects of age and word characteristics , 1990 .

[106]  John R. Anderson The Adaptive Character of Thought , 1990 .

[107]  Peter Bryant,et al.  Rhyme and Alliteration, Phoneme Detection, and Learning to Read. , 1990 .

[108]  R Treiman,et al.  Subsyllabic units in computerized reading instruction: onset-rime vs. postvowel segmentation. , 1990, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[109]  R. Treiman,et al.  Units in reading and spelling , 1988 .

[110]  M. Maclean Rhymes, nursery rhymes, and reading in early childhood. , 1987 .

[111]  C. S. Wallace,et al.  Estimation and Inference by Compact Coding , 1987 .

[112]  R. Wagner,et al.  The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. , 1987 .

[113]  James A. Reggia,et al.  Empirically derived probabilities for grapheme-to-phoneme correspondences in english , 1987 .

[114]  F. Vellutino,et al.  Experimental Evidence for the Effects of Instructional Bias on Word Identification , 1986, Exceptional children.

[115]  U. Goswami Children's use of analogy in learning to read: A developmental study , 1986 .

[116]  G. E. MacKinnon,et al.  Reading Research Advances in Theory and Practice , 1985 .

[117]  Franklin R. Manis,et al.  Acquisition of word identification skills in normal and disabled readers , 1985 .

[118]  F. Morrison Reading disability: A problem in rule learning and word decoding , 1984 .

[119]  L. Henderson Orthography and Word Recognition in Reading , 1982 .

[120]  G. Miller,et al.  Cognitive science. , 1981, Science.

[121]  P. Bryant,et al.  Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading backwardness , 1978, Nature.

[122]  Vladimir I. Levenshtein,et al.  Binary codes capable of correcting deletions, insertions, and reversals , 1965 .