How task errors affect subsequent behavior: Evidence from distributional analyses of task-switching effects
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] James L. McClelland,et al. On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.
[2] D. Norman,et al. Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .
[3] N. Meiran,et al. Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[4] N. Lavie. Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[5] D. Norman,et al. Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .
[6] Nachshon Meiran,et al. Advance task preparation reduces task error rate in the cuing task-switching paradigm , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[7] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] E. Viding,et al. Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[9] G. Logan. Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .
[10] B. Hommel. Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] C. Eriksen,et al. Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[12] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[13] D. Alan Allport,et al. SHIFTING INTENTIONAL SET - EXPLORING THE DYNAMIC CONTROL OF TASKS , 1994 .
[14] Martin E. Maier,et al. How Stress Affects Strategies of Task-Set Reconfiguration , 2007 .
[15] D. Spalding. The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.
[16] Andrea M Philipp,et al. Response selection and response execution in task switching: evidence from a go-signal paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[17] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Response inhibition in conflict tasks is revealed in delta plots , 2004 .
[18] Erik M Altmann,et al. Repetition priming in task switching: Do the benefits dissipate? , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[19] Marco Steinhauser,et al. Response-based strengthening in task shifting: evidence from shift effects produced by errors. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] James L. McClelland. Failures to Learn and their Remediation: A Hebbian Account , 1999 .
[21] M. Steinhauser,et al. Automatic activation of task-related representations in task shifting , 2007, Memory & cognition.
[22] Barbara A. Wilson,et al. When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem of error elimination , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[23] Marco Steinhauser,et al. Modeling behavioral measures of error detection in choice tasks: response monitoring versus conflict monitoring. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] N. Meiran. Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .
[25] D. Laming. Choice reaction performance following an error , 1979 .
[26] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[27] M. Moscovitch,et al. Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .
[28] I. Koch,et al. The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] P M Rabbitt,et al. Three Kinds of Error-Signalling Responses in a Serial Choice Task , 1968, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[30] Marco Steinhauser,et al. Mixing costs in task shifting reflect sequential processing stages in a multicomponent task , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[31] H. Fairweather,et al. Choice reaction times in children: Error and post-error responses, and the repetition effect , 1978 .
[32] F. Lhermitte. 'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[33] Walter Schneider,et al. Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .
[34] R. Proctor,et al. Mixing compatible and incompatible mappings: Elimination, reduction, and enhancement of spatial compatibility effects , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[35] V. Bruce. Unsolved mysteries of the mind : tutorial essays in cognition , 1998 .
[36] J. Shaoul. Human Error , 1973, Nature.
[37] D. Norman. Categorization of action slips. , 1981 .
[38] R. Proctor,et al. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[39] R Hübner,et al. On attentional control as a source of residual shift costs: evidence from two-component task shifts. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[40] N. Meiran,et al. On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[41] N. Meiran. Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.
[42] R De Jong,et al. An intention-activation account of residual switch costs , 2000 .
[43] On attentional control as a source of residual shift costs: evidence from two-component task shifts. , 2001 .
[44] K Richard Ridderinkhof,et al. Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/Hd that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. , 2005, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[45] Richard Ridderinkhof. Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks , 2002, Psychological research.