When less is more: Structural correlates of core executive functions in young adults – A VBM and cortical thickness study

Background: The term executive functions (EF) describes a set of higher cognitive abilities/skills needed for goal‐oriented and flexible behavior. In contrast to a multitude of functional neuroimaging studies of EF performance, only limited and partially inconclusive data is available for the structural‐neuroanatomical underpinnings of EFs, particularly in healthy adults. Methods: Here, we applied voxel‐based morphometry (VBM) and additional analyses of cortical thickness (CTH; via surface‐based morphometry) to a large sample of healthy young adults from the Human Connectome Project (N=1110; Age 28.8±3.7 years) with structural MRI data and test data reflective of three core EFs [i.e. cognitive flexibility (CF), inhibitory control (IC) and working memory (WM)]. Results: For CF and IC, VBM analyses yielded a distinct and largely overlapping pattern of exclusively negative associations (CF>IC), most prominently within the medial prefrontal cortex, the insular cortex, central/precentral regions, subcortical and mesotemporal structures. A similar, yet less pronounced pattern of negative associations was found in analyses of CTH. In contrast, both VBM and CTH analyses yielded no significant associations with WM performance. Conclusions: Brain regions we found negatively associated with measures of CF and IC have been repeatedly highlighted by functional imaging studies of EF performance. The here observed inverse relationship with brain structural parameters may be related to the young age of our study population and well established neurobiological mechanisms of cortical maturation (i.e. cortical thinning via synaptic pruning and cortical myelination). HIGHLIGHTSVBM and cortical thickness in a large sample of young healthy adults.Structural correlates of different tests reflective of core executive functions.Inverse associations of gray matter volume with better test performance.Inverse associations of cortical thickness with better test performance.

[1]  Diane Swick,et al.  Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks , 2011, NeuroImage.

[2]  N. Fox,et al.  NIH Toolbox for Assessment of Neurological and Behavioral Function , 2013, Neurology.

[3]  Sam Goldstein,et al.  Introduction: A History of Executive Functioning as a Theoretical and Clinical Construct , 2014 .

[4]  G. Shields,et al.  Executive function performance in obesity and overweight individuals: A meta-analysis and review , 2018, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[5]  L. Westlye,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of executive functions in children and adolescents: A magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study of cortical thickness , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Hannah R. Snyder Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: a meta-analysis and review. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  Essa Yacoub,et al.  The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: An overview , 2013, NeuroImage.

[8]  D. Tulsky,et al.  V. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring working memory. , 2013, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[9]  H. Werner,et al.  Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. , 1962, The Journal of genetic psychology.

[10]  Alfred W. Kaszniak,et al.  Executive Control Functions in Degenerative Dementias: A Comparative Review , 2000, Neuropsychology Review.

[11]  CHARLES E. Bailey Cognitive Accuracy and Intelligent Executive Function in the Brain and in Business , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[12]  J. Pekar,et al.  Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents , 2006, Nature.

[14]  Joseph M. Orr,et al.  Individual differences in regional prefrontal gray matter morphometry and fractional anisotropy are associated with different constructs of executive function , 2014, Brain Structure and Function.

[15]  Richard J Hodes,et al.  The NIH Toolbox , 2013, Neurology.

[16]  Jesper Tegnér,et al.  Stronger Synaptic Connectivity as a Mechanism behind Development of Working Memory-related Brain Activity during Childhood , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Prefrontal activation due to Stroop interference increases during development—an event-related fNIRS study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[18]  Peter F. Liddle,et al.  Differential effects of surface area, gyrification and cortical thickness on voxel based morphometric deficits in schizophrenia , 2012, NeuroImage.

[19]  Kimberly L. Ray,et al.  Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions , 2012, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[20]  Hans Knutsson,et al.  Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  S. MacDonald,et al.  Intra-individual variability in behavior: links to brain structure, neurotransmission and neuronal activity , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[22]  N. Fox,et al.  Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox , 2013, Neurology.

[23]  V. Menon,et al.  Neural basis of protracted developmental changes in visuo-spatial working memory , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  J. Fletcher,et al.  Developmental changes in performance on tests of purported frontal lobe functioning , 1991 .

[25]  K. Miller Executive functions. , 2005, Pediatric annals.

[26]  Irene E. Nagel,et al.  Cortical thickness is linked to executive functioning in adulthood and aging , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[27]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference , 2009, NeuroImage.

[28]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  A comparison between voxel-based cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry in normal aging , 2009, NeuroImage.

[29]  Sandra Weintraub,et al.  II. NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring executive function and attention. , 2013, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[30]  N. Raz,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: A meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies , 2014, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[31]  Christian Gaser,et al.  Cortical thickness and central surface estimation , 2013, NeuroImage.

[32]  K. Berman,et al.  Meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin Card‐Sorting task and component processes , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[33]  Steen Moeller,et al.  The Human Connectome Project: A data acquisition perspective , 2012, NeuroImage.

[34]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Global associations between regional gray matter volume and diverse complex cognitive functions: evidence from a large sample study , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[35]  Kathryn M. McMillan,et al.  N‐back working memory paradigm: A meta‐analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[36]  T. Alloway,et al.  Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. , 2010, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[37]  J. Lichtman,et al.  From Cajal to Connectome and Beyond. , 2016, Annual review of neuroscience.

[38]  G. Šimić,et al.  Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  Bogdan Draganski,et al.  Training-induced structural changes in the adult human brain , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[40]  C. Boettiger,et al.  Impulsivity, frontal lobes and risk for addiction , 2009, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[41]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Mapping Human Cortical Areas In Vivo Based on Myelin Content as Revealed by T1- and T2-Weighted MRI , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  Yasuyuki Taki,et al.  Brain structures associated with executive functions during everyday events in a non-clinical sample , 2012, Brain Structure and Function.

[43]  M. Willoughby,et al.  Executive Function Buffers the Association between Early Math and Later Academic Skills , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[44]  B. Gold,et al.  Domain general and domain preferential brain regions associated with different types of task switching: A Meta‐Analysis , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[45]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  The human connectome: Origins and challenges , 2013, NeuroImage.

[46]  Ameera X. Patel,et al.  Waves of Maturation and Senescence in Micro-structural MRI Markers of Human Cortical Myelination over the Lifespan , 2018, bioRxiv.

[47]  Yajun Wang,et al.  Quantitative prediction of individual cognitive flexibility using structural MRI , 2018, Brain Imaging and Behavior.

[48]  R. Dauman,et al.  Intra-individual variability in tinnitus patients , 2015, HNO.

[49]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Voxel-Based Morphometry—The Methods , 2000, NeuroImage.

[50]  Peter B. Jones,et al.  373. Adolescence is Associated with Genomically Patterned Consolidation of the Hubs of the Human Brain Connectome , 2016, Biological Psychiatry.