Construct accessibility and depression: an examination of cognitive and affective factors.

Two studies in which the parameters of construct accessibility in depression were examined are reported. In Experiment 1, depressed and nondepressed subjects were required to name the colors of tachistiscopically presented depressed-content, neutral-content, and manic-content words. Because of the predicted accessibility and interference effects of the depressed-content words, the depressed subjects were expected to demonstrate longer response latencies to these words than to the non-depressed-content words. This response pattern was found for the depressed subjects; the nondepressed subjects did not demonstrate differential response latencies. In Experiment 2, a mood-induction paradigm was used to investigate whether the interference effects obtained in Experiment 1 were due to temporary mood differences between the depressed and nondepressed subjects, or were a function of more stable depression-associated patterns of information processing. Although predicted group differences were obtained on a mood adjective checklist, no effects were found for task performance. These results suggest that transient mood is not a sufficient explanation for the results obtained in Experiment 1. The implications of the present findings for the understanding of both construct accessibility and depression are discussed, and directions for future research are suggested.

[1]  I. Gotlib Depression and general psychopathology in university students. , 1984, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[2]  J. Coyne,et al.  The role of cognition in depression: a critical appraisal. , 1983, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  N. Kuiper,et al.  Self and Other Perception in Mild Depressives , 1982 .

[4]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Responses to depressed individuals: discrepancies between self-report and observer-rated behavior. , 1982, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[5]  E. Higgins,et al.  Individual construct accessibility and subjective impressions and recall. , 1982 .

[6]  M. Natale,et al.  Effect of temporary mood states on selective memory about the self. , 1982 .

[7]  W. S. Rholes,et al.  The Velten mood induction procedure: effects on mood and memory. , 1982, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[8]  I. Gotlib Self-reinforcement and depression in interpersonal interaction: the role of performance level. , 1982, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[9]  Lyn Y. Abramson,et al.  Induced mood and the illusion of control. , 1981 .

[10]  I. Gotlib Self-reinforcement and recall: differential deficits in depressed and nondepressed psychiatric inpatients. , 1981, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[11]  N. Kuiper,et al.  Schematic processing and self-reference in clinical depression. , 1981, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[12]  J. Coyne,et al.  Demand characteristics and the Velten mood induction procedure. , 1981 .

[13]  Robert A. Taylor,et al.  Induced mood and accessibility of memories: An effect of mood state or of induction procedure? , 1981 .

[14]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Category accessibility: Some theoretical and empirical issues concerning the processing of social stimulus information , 1981 .

[15]  G. Bower Mood and memory. , 1981, The American psychologist.

[16]  David C. Rubin,et al.  51 properties of 125 words: A unit analysis of verbal behavior , 1980 .

[17]  J. Polivy,et al.  Laboratory induction of mood states through the reading of self-referent mood statements: affective changes or demand characteristics? , 1980, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[18]  R. Zajonc Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .

[19]  W. Chaplin,et al.  Social competence and depression: the role of illusory self-perceptions. , 1980, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[20]  M. Kovács Cognitive therapy in depression. , 1980, The Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis.

[21]  L. Abramson,et al.  Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: sadder but wiser? , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[22]  R. Frost,et al.  Self-devaluation and depressed mood. , 1979, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[23]  F. Terrell,et al.  The illusion of control among depressed patients. , 1979, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[24]  J. Teasdale,et al.  Differential effects of induced mood on retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic memory. , 1979, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[25]  R. Asarnow,et al.  Interpersonal and impersonal problem-solving skills in mildly and clinically depressed university students. , 1979, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[26]  G. W. Bradley Self-serving biases in the attribution process: A reexamination of the fact or fiction question. , 1978 .

[27]  R. Depue,et al.  Learned helplessness in the perspective of the depressive disorders: conceptual and definitional issues. , 1978, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[28]  A. Isen,et al.  Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: a cognitive loop? , 1978, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  R. E. Nelson,et al.  Selective recall of positive and negative feedback, self-control behaviors, and depression. , 1977, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[30]  A. M. Buchwald Depressive mood and estimates of reinforcement frequency. , 1977, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[31]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[32]  B. Jaffe Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders , 1977 .

[33]  T. Dorpat Depressive affect. , 1977, The Psychoanalytic study of the child.

[34]  F. Terrell,et al.  Depression and the illusion of control. , 1977, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[35]  M. Natale Effects of induced elation-depression on speech in the initial interview. , 1977, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[36]  W. S. Rholes,et al.  Category accessibility and impression formation , 1977 .

[37]  B. Strickland,et al.  Induction of mood states and their effect on cognitive and social behaviors. , 1976 .

[38]  J. Coyne,et al.  Toward an interactional description of depression. , 1976, Psychiatry.

[39]  The Induction of Mood States and Their Effect on Cognitive and Social Behaviors , 1974, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[40]  A E Wener,et al.  Depressive affect: a test of behavioral hypotheses. , 1975, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[41]  Dale T. Miller,et al.  Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: Fact or fiction? , 1975 .

[42]  F. Craik,et al.  Depth of processing and the retention of words , 1975 .

[43]  Edward Sagarin,et al.  Humanistic Psychotherapy: The Rational-Emotive Approach , 1974 .

[44]  B. Strickland,et al.  Effect of Induced Mood States on Activity and Self Reported Affect , 1974 .

[45]  P. Lewinsohn,et al.  A behavioral approach to depression. , 1974 .

[46]  R. E. Warren,et al.  Association, directionality, and stimulus encoding. , 1974 .

[47]  Carol Conrad,et al.  Context effects in sentence comprehension: A study of the subjective lexicon , 1974, Memory & cognition.

[48]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of Pro-cessing: A Framework for Memory Research , 1975 .

[49]  R. E. Warren,et al.  Stimulus encoding and memory. , 1972 .

[50]  C. G. Costello Depression: Loss of reinforcers of loss of reinforcer effectiveness? , 1972 .

[51]  E. Tulving,et al.  Episodic and semantic memory , 1972 .

[52]  E. Velten A laboratory task for induction of mood states. , 1968, Behaviour research and therapy.

[53]  A. Beck,et al.  Depression: Causes and Treatment , 1967 .

[54]  M. Orne On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. , 1962 .

[55]  A. Beck,et al.  An inventory for measuring depression. , 1961, Archives of general psychiatry.

[56]  J. Bruner On perceptual readiness. , 1957, Psychological review.

[57]  P. Witty The teacher's word book of 30,000 words. , 1945 .