Help me if I can't: Social interaction effects in adult contextual word learning
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Cordula Vesper,et al. Making oneself predictable: reduced temporal variability facilitates joint action coordination , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] Kenji Hakuta,et al. Confounded age: Linguistic and cognitive factors in age differences for second language acquisition. , 1999 .
[3] H. Walter,et al. Do you mean me? Communicative intentions recruit the mirror and the mentalizing system. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[4] H. Gleitman,et al. The current status of the motherese hypothesis , 1984, Journal of Child Language.
[5] N. Rader,et al. Caregivers’ gestures direct infant attention during early word learning: the importance of dynamic synchrony , 2012 .
[6] Jacqueline Nadel,et al. From social behaviour to brain synchronization: Review and perspectives in hyperscanning , 2011 .
[7] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Timed Action and Object Naming , 2005, Cortex.
[8] Henk G Schmidt,et al. Detrimental influence of contextual change on spacing effects in free recall. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[9] J. Rączaszek-Leonardi,et al. Pooling the ground: understanding and coordination in collective sense making , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[10] Peter Ford Dominey,et al. Indeterminacy in language acquisition: the role of child directed speech and joint attention , 2004, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[11] T. Bongaerts,et al. Authenticity of pronunciation in naturalistic second language acquisition: The case of very advanced late learners of Dutch as a second language , 2000 .
[12] Michael J Kahana,et al. Contextual Variability in Free Recall. , 2011, Journal of memory and language.
[13] H. Bekkering,et al. Exploring the brain basis of joint action: Co-ordination of actions, goals and intentions , 2007, Social neuroscience.
[14] D. Perani,et al. Neural convergence for language comprehension and grammatical class production in highly proficient bilinguals is independent of age of acquisition , 2013, Cortex.
[15] Line Garnero,et al. Inter-Brain Synchronization during Social Interaction , 2010, PloS one.
[16] William E. Nagy,et al. Learning Word Meanings From Context During Normal Reading , 1987 .
[17] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Timed picture naming: Extended norms and validation against previous studies , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[18] R. Malach,et al. Intersubject Synchronization of Cortical Activity During Natural Vision , 2004, Science.
[19] F. Bellezza,et al. Encoding variability, memory organization, and the repetition effect. , 1982 .
[20] Kerstin Fischer,et al. Mindful tutors: Linguistic choice and action demonstration in speech to infants and a simulated robot , 2011 .
[21] Gordon D. A. Brown,et al. Contextual Diversity, Not Word Frequency, Determines Word-Naming and Lexical Decision Times , 2006, Psychological science.
[22] Paula Fitzpatrick,et al. Understanding social motor coordination. , 2011, Human movement science.
[23] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Timed picture naming in seven languages , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[24] L. Gogate,et al. A study of multimodal motherese: the role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. , 2000, Child development.
[25] J. Watson,et al. Early socio–emotional development: Contingency perception and the social-biofeedback model. , 1999 .
[26] Y. Yamaguchi,et al. Inter-brain synchronization during coordination of speech rhythm in human-to-human social interaction , 2013, Scientific Reports.
[27] J. Kelso,et al. Binding of movement, sound and touch: multimodal coordination dynamics , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] Thomas T. Hills,et al. The Associative Structure of Language: Contextual Diversity in Early Word Learning. , 2010, Journal of memory and language.
[29] F. N. Dempster,et al. Effects of variable encoding and spaced presentations on vocabulary learning. , 1987 .
[30] Martin J. Pickering,et al. Alignment as the Basis for Successful Communication , 2006 .
[31] S. Huettel,et al. A nexus model of the temporal–parietal junction , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[32] M. Pickering,et al. The interactive-alignment model: Developments and refinements , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[33] A. Baddeley,et al. Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: on land and underwater. , 1975 .
[34] C. F. Bond,et al. Social facilitation: a meta-analysis of 241 studies. , 1983, Psychological bulletin.
[35] S. Riek,et al. The effect of attention on the release of anticipatory timing actions. , 2014, Behavioral neuroscience.
[36] U. Hasson,et al. Speaker–listener neural coupling underlies successful communication , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[37] Katharina J. Rohlfing,et al. Attention via Synchrony: Making Use of Multimodal Cues in Social Learning , 2009, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.
[38] Michael Tomasello,et al. The social-pragmatic theory of word learning. , 2000 .
[39] S. Kotz,et al. Play along: effects of music and social interaction on word learning , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[40] Margaret Wilson,et al. An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[41] A. Glenberg,et al. Component-levels theory of the effects of spacing of repetitions on recall and recognition , 1979, Memory & cognition.
[42] S. Cobb. Speech and Brain-Mechanisms. , 1960 .
[43] C. Frith,et al. The Neural Basis of Mentalizing , 2006, Neuron.
[44] P. Keller,et al. Neural correlates of auditory temporal predictions during sensorimotor synchronization , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[45] A. Glenberg. Monotonic and nonmonotonic lag effects in paired-associate and recognition memory paradigms , 1976 .
[46] L. Fadiga,et al. Lexicality drives audio-motor transformations in Broca’s area , 2010, Brain and Language.
[47] L. Gogate,et al. Intersensory Redundancy and 7-Month-Old Infants' Memory for Arbitrary Syllable-Object Relations , 2001 .
[48] M. Legerstee,et al. Contingency, imitation, and affect sharing: Foundations of infants' social awareness. , 2006, Developmental psychology.
[49] C. Frith,et al. Mechanisms of social cognition. , 2012, Annual review of psychology.
[50] Shigeru Sato,et al. Learning second language vocabulary: Neural dissociation of situation-based learning and text-based learning , 2010, NeuroImage.
[51] Alexander P. Demos,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology : General Rocking to the Beat : Effects of Music and Partner ' s Movements on Spontaneous Interpersonal Coordination , 2011 .
[52] Rick Dale,et al. Behavior Matching in Multimodal Communication Is Synchronized , 2012, Cogn. Sci..
[53] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[54] J. Decety,et al. The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[55] Morris Moscovitch,et al. Depth of processing, retrieval cues, and uniqueness of encoding as factors in recall , 1976 .
[56] Linda B. Smith. Learning How to Learn Words , 2000 .
[57] Nadim Joni Shah,et al. Minds Made for Sharing: Initiating Joint Attention Recruits Reward-related Neurocircuitry , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[58] Gereon R Fink,et al. The right temporoparietal junction in attention and social interaction: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2016, Human brain mapping.
[59] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Social gating and pedagogy: Mechanisms for learning and implications for robotics , 2010, Neural Networks.
[60] N. Triplett,et al. The Dynamogenic Factors in Pacemaking and Competition , 1898 .
[61] G. Fink,et al. It's in your eyes--using gaze-contingent stimuli to create truly interactive paradigms for social cognitive and affective neuroscience. , 2010, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[62] Gian Luca Romani,et al. Being an agent or an observer: Different spectral dynamics revealed by MEG , 2014, NeuroImage.
[63] G. Csibra,et al. Natural pedagogy , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[64] Msl Swanborn,et al. Incidental Word Learning While Reading: A Meta-Analysis , 1999 .
[65] J. Hulstijn,et al. INCIDENTAL VOCABULARY ACQUISITION IN A SECOND LANGUAGE: THE CONSTRUCT OF TASK-INDUCED INVOLVEMENT , 2001 .
[66] D. Caplan,et al. Processing of Visually Presented Sentences in Mandarin and English Studied with fMRI , 1999, Neuron.
[67] Eraldo Paulesu,et al. The role of age of acquisition and language usage in early, high‐proficient bilinguals: An fMRI study during verbal fluency , 2003, Human brain mapping.
[68] Cordula Vesper,et al. Are you ready to jump? Predictive mechanisms in interpersonal coordination. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[69] P. Kuhl,et al. Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[70] P. Kay,et al. Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[71] K. Vogeley,et al. Toward a second-person neuroscience 1 , 2013, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[72] L. Schilbach,et al. On the relationship of online and offline social cognition , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[73] Jessica S. Horst,et al. The role of competition in word learning via referent selection. , 2010, Developmental science.
[74] Ulrika Kahl,et al. [The bilingual brain]. , 2002, Lakartidningen.
[75] J. Bruner. From communication to language—a psychological perspective , 1975, Cognition.
[76] T. Striano,et al. Sensitivity to triadic attention in early infancy. , 2005, Developmental science.
[77] Michael Tomasello,et al. Two-Year-Olds Use Pragmatic Cues to Differentiate Reference to Objects and Actions , 1995 .
[78] V Bettinardi,et al. The bilingual brain. Proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[79] S. Kotz,et al. How relevant is social interaction in second language learning? , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[80] P. Kuhl. Is speech learning 'gated' by the social brain? , 2007, Developmental science.
[81] C. Frith,et al. Language as a Tool for Interacting Minds , 2010 .
[82] M. Chun,et al. Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.
[83] Rebecca J. Brand,et al. Breaking the language barrier: an emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. , 2000, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
[84] Kenneth J. Malmberg,et al. The effect of normative context variability on recognition memory. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[85] Barry S. Stein,et al. Depth of processing reexamined: The effects of the precision of encoding and test appropriateness , 1978 .
[86] S. Garrod,et al. Brain-to-brain coupling: a mechanism for creating and sharing a social world , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[87] Markus Conci,et al. Contextual learning of multiple target locations in visual search , 2012 .
[88] M. Strube,et al. What didTriplett really find? A contemporary analysis of the first experiment in social psychology. , 2005, The American journal of psychology.
[89] Larissa K. Samuelson,et al. Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning. , 2012, Psychological review.
[90] E. Tulving,et al. Relation between encoding specificity and levels of precessing , 1979 .
[91] M. Jackson. What do you mean? , 1989, Geriatric nursing.
[92] L. Gogate,et al. The intersensory origins of word‐comprehension: an ecological–dynamic systems view , 2001 .
[93] R. Zajonc. SOCIAL FACILITATION. , 1965, Science.
[94] F. Craik,et al. Levels of Pro-cessing: A Framework for Memory Research , 1975 .
[95] Daniel O. Jackson,et al. Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis , 2000 .
[96] S. Waxman,et al. Early word-learning entails reference, not merely associations , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[97] Sebo Uithol,et al. Engagement: Looking beyond the mirror to understand action understanding. , 2016, The British journal of developmental psychology.
[98] Stefano F. Cappa,et al. The bilingual brain as revealed by functional neuroimaging , 2001, The Bilingualism Reader.
[99] O. Reiser,et al. Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .
[100] Linda B. Smith,et al. Social coordination in toddler's word learning: interacting systems of perception and action , 2008, Connect. Sci..
[101] Jubin Abutalebi,et al. Neural aspects of second language representation and language control. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[102] Cathy J Price,et al. Convergence, degeneracy and control. , 2006, Language learning.
[103] B. Repp. Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[104] J. Elman,et al. Learning to use words: Event-related potentials index single-shot contextual word learning , 2010, Cognition.
[105] Katharina J. Rohlfing,et al. Is ostension any more than attention? , 2014, Scientific Reports.
[106] Relation Between Encoding Specificity and Levels of Processing , 2014 .
[107] Larissa K. Samuelson,et al. Learn Locally, Think Globally , 2010, Psychological science.
[108] A. Rodríguez-Fornells,et al. Neurophysiological mechanisms involved in language learning in adults , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[109] M. Tomasello,et al. Shared intentionality. , 2007, Developmental science.
[110] C. Snow. Mothers' Speech to Children Learning Language. , 1972 .
[111] Michael J. Richardson,et al. Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. , 2007, Human movement science.
[112] R. Marsh,et al. An observation on the role of context variability in free recall. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[113] Sean M. Polyn,et al. A context maintenance and retrieval model of organizational processes in free recall. , 2009, Psychological review.
[114] J. Kelso,et al. Social coordination dynamics: Measuring human bonding , 2008, Social neuroscience.
[115] Jochen Kaiser,et al. Auditory–motor synchronization facilitates attention allocation , 2013, NeuroImage.
[116] Ellen M. Markman,et al. Constraints Children Place on Word Meanings , 1990, Cogn. Sci..
[117] Chaozhe Zhu,et al. Neural Synchronization during Face-to-Face Communication , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.