Useful Field of View Predicts Driving in the Presence of Distracters

Purpose. The Useful Field of View (UFOV®) test has been shown to be highly effective in predicting crash risk among older adults. An important question which we examined in this study is whether this association is due to the ability of the UFOV to predict difficulties in attention-demanding driving situations that involve either visual or auditory distracters. Methods. Participants included 92 community-living adults (mean age 73.6 ± 5.4 years; range 65–88 years) who completed all three subtests of the UFOV involving assessment of visual processing speed (subtest 1), divided attention (subtest 2), and selective attention (subtest 3); driving safety risk was also classified using the UFOV scoring system. Driving performance was assessed separately on a closed-road circuit while driving under three conditions: no distracters, visual distracters, and auditory distracters. Driving outcome measures included road sign recognition, hazard detection, gap perception, time to complete the course, and performance on the distracter tasks. Results. Those rated as safe on the UFOV (safety rating categories 1 and 2), as well as those responding faster than the recommended cut-off on the selective attention subtest (350 msec), performed significantly better in terms of overall driving performance and also experienced less interference from distracters. Of the three UFOV subtests, the selective attention subtest best predicted overall driving performance in the presence of distracters. Conclusions. Older adults who were rated as higher risk on the UFOV, particularly on the selective attention subtest, demonstrated poorest driving performance in the presence of distracters. This finding suggests that the selective attention subtest of the UFOV may be differentially more effective in predicting driving difficulties in situations of divided attention which are commonly associated with crashes.

[1]  Rua Fernando Osório neuropsychologIcal tests and drIvIng In deMentIa: a revIew of the recent lIterature , 2009 .

[2]  Thomas J Triggs,et al.  Driver distraction: the effects of concurrent in-vehicle tasks, road environment complexity and age on driving performance. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[3]  Alex Chaparro,et al.  Interaction between visual status, driver age and distracters on daytime driving performance , 2009, Vision Research.

[4]  Bruce J. Avolio,et al.  Individual Differences in Information-Processing Ability as a Predictor of Motor Vehicle Accidents , 1985 .

[5]  G McGwin,et al.  Characteristics of traffic crashes among young, middle-aged, and older drivers. , 1999, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[6]  David D Clarke,et al.  Older drivers' road traffic crashes in the UK. , 2010, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[7]  Lana M Trick,et al.  The effects of visibility conditions, traffic density, and navigational challenge on speed compensation and driving performance in older adults. , 2010, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[8]  Sonia Amado,et al.  The effects of conversation on attention and peripheral detection: Is talking with a passenger and talking on the cell phone different? , 2005 .

[9]  Normand Teasdale,et al.  Mental workload when driving in a simulator: effects of age and driving complexity. , 2009, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[10]  J. Wood,et al.  Aging, driving and vision , 2002, Clinical & experimental optometry.

[11]  Cheryl L Grady,et al.  Changes in memory processing with age , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[12]  D. C. Howell Statistical methods for psychology, 3rd ed. , 1992 .

[13]  Karlene K. Ball,et al.  Speed-of-Processing and Driving Simulator Training Result in Improved Driving Performance , 2003, Hum. Factors.

[14]  E Keskinen,et al.  Older drivers fail in intersections: speed discrepancies between older and younger male drivers. , 1998, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[15]  Alex Chaparro,et al.  Effects of Age and Auditory and Visual Dual-Tasks on Closed Road Driving Performance , 2004, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[16]  Paul Atchley,et al.  Conversation Limits the Functional Field of View , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[17]  M. Crotty,et al.  Establishing criterion validity of the Useful Field of View assessment and Stroke Drivers' Screening Assessment: comparison to the result of on-road assessment. , 2010, The American journal of occupational therapy : official publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association.

[18]  Karen Bandeen-Roche,et al.  A prospective, population-based study of the role of visual impairment in motor vehicle crashes among older drivers: the SEE study. , 2007, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[19]  L. A. Chiasson,et al.  Risk of falls and motor vehicle collisions in glaucoma. , 2007, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[20]  K. Ball,et al.  Visual/cognitive correlates of vehicle accidents in older drivers. , 1991, Psychology and aging.

[21]  Alex Chaparro,et al.  Hearing Impairment Affects Older People's Ability to Drive in the Presence of Distracters , 2010, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[22]  Marlos Viana,et al.  SHORT NOTE Relative Effects of Age and Compromised Vision on Driving Performance , 1995, Hum. Factors.

[23]  Karlene K. Ball,et al.  Reliability and Validity of Useful Field of View Test Scores as Administered by Personal Computer , 2005, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[24]  O. Shechtman,et al.  Useful Field of View as a Reliable Screening Measure of Driving Performance in People With Parkinson's Disease: Results of a Pilot Study , 2009, Traffic injury prevention.

[25]  Thomas A. Dingus,et al.  The Impact of Driver Inattention on Near-Crash/Crash Risk: An Analysis Using the 100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study Data , 2006 .

[26]  Kaarin J Anstey,et al.  Chronological age and age-related cognitive deficits are associated with an increase in multiple types of driving errors in late life. , 2011, Neuropsychology.

[27]  Gayla M. Cissell,et al.  Can High‐Risk Older Drivers Be Identified Through Performance‐Based Measures in a Department of Motor Vehicles Setting? , 2006, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[28]  Loren S. Groff,et al.  Maintaining Situational Awareness: The Role of Visual Attention , 1999 .

[29]  Gayla M. Cissell,et al.  The longitudinal impact of cognitive speed of processing training on driving mobility. , 2009, The Gerontologist.

[30]  J. Wood,et al.  Age and Visual Impairment Decrease Driving Performance as Measured on a Closed-Road Circuit , 2002, Hum. Factors.

[31]  D. Roth,et al.  Cumulative Meta-analysis of the Relationship Between Useful Field of View and Driving Performance in Older Adults: Current and Future Implications , 2005, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[32]  M. Reger,et al.  The relationship between neuropsychological functioning and driving ability in dementia: a meta-analysis. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[33]  A. Wingfield,et al.  Hearing Loss in Older Adulthood , 2005 .

[34]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View , 1988 .

[35]  C. Lustig,et al.  Inhibitory Mechanisms and the Control of Attention , 2007 .

[36]  A. Baddeley Working memory: looking back and looking forward , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[37]  P. Nestor,et al.  Attention and Driving Skills in Aging and Alzheimer's Disease , 1991, Human factors.

[38]  L Hakamies-Blomqvist,et al.  Older drivers' accident risk: conceptual and methodological issues. , 1998, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[39]  G S Rubin,et al.  Visual and medical risk factors for motor vehicle collision involvement among older drivers , 2008, British Journal of Ophthalmology.

[40]  Suzanne P McEvoy,et al.  The contribution of passengers versus mobile phone use to motor vehicle crashes resulting in hospital attendance by the driver. , 2007, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[41]  E. Engelhardt,et al.  Neuropsychological tests and driving in dementia: a review of the recent literature. , 2009, Revista da Associacao Medica Brasileira.

[42]  A F Williams,et al.  Fatal crash risk for older drivers at intersections. , 1998, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[43]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[44]  K Ball,et al.  The Role of Selective Attention in Driving and Dementia of the Alzheimer Type , 1997, Alzheimer disease and associated disorders.

[45]  J. Duchek,et al.  Attention and driving performance in Alzheimer's disease. , 1998, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[46]  M. Sloane,et al.  Visual processing impairment and risk of motor vehicle crash among older adults. , 1998, JAMA.

[47]  R. Tibshirani,et al.  Association between cellular-telephone calls and motor vehicle collisions. , 1997, The New England journal of medicine.

[48]  H. Pashler Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[49]  N. Raz Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings. , 2000 .

[50]  D. Roth,et al.  Relation of Useful Field of View and other Screening Tests to on-Road Driving Performance , 2000, Perceptual and motor skills.

[51]  K. Ball,et al.  Visual attention problems as a predictor of vehicle crashes in older drivers. , 1993, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[52]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[53]  John D. Lee,et al.  Traffic-Entry Behavior and Crash Risk for Older Drivers with Impairment of Selective Attention , 2006, Perceptual and motor skills.