Uncovering the architecture of action semantics.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Brett,et al. Actions Speak Louder Than Functions: The Importance of Manipulability and Action in Tool Representation , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[2] M. Jeannerod. Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.
[3] S. Cappa,et al. The breakdown of semantic knowledge: Insights from a statistical model of meaning representation , 2003, Brain and Language.
[4] Alex Martin,et al. Grounding Object Concepts in Perception and Action: Evidence from FMRI Studies of Tools , 2007, Cortex.
[5] L. Buxbaum,et al. Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems , 2010, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[6] L. Buxbaum,et al. Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[7] G. Lakoff,et al. The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[8] Friedemann Pulverm Uuml,et al. Words in the brain's language , 1999 .
[9] George S. Cree,et al. Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words , 2008, Cognition.
[10] W. Levelt,et al. Semantic distance effects on object and action naming , 2002, Cognition.
[11] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content , 2008, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[12] W. K. Simmons,et al. A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[13] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[14] Susan J. Lederman,et al. Cognitive representations of functional interactions with objects , 1993, Memory & cognition.
[15] David C. Plaut,et al. Semantic and Associative Priming in a Distributed Attractor Network , 1995 .
[16] G. Rizzolatti,et al. The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.
[17] A. Glenberg,et al. Processing Abstract Language Modulates Motor System Activity , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[18] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Lateralization of object-shape information in semantic processing , 2004, Cognition.
[19] Laurel J Buxbaum,et al. Critical brain regions for action recognition: lesion symptom mapping in left hemisphere stroke. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[20] Marc W Howard,et al. When Does Semantic Similarity Help Episodic Retrieval , 2002 .
[21] Siddharth Suri,et al. Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk , 2010, Behavior research methods.
[22] D. V. von Cramon,et al. Separable neuronal circuitries for manipulable and non-manipulable objects in working memory. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.
[23] Ian S. Hargreaves,et al. The neural correlates of the body-object interaction effect in semantic processing , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[24] Julie C. Sedivy,et al. Impaired access to manipulation features in Apraxia: Evidence from eyetracking and semantic judgment tasks , 2010, Brain and Language.
[25] M. Schwartz,et al. Semantic interference during blocked-cyclic naming: Evidence from aphasia , 2006 .
[26] W. Glaser,et al. Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[27] W. Levelt,et al. Effects of semantic context in the naming of pictures and words , 2001, Cognition.
[28] Eva Belke. Effects of working memory load on lexical-semantic encoding in language production , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[29] B. P. McCloskey,et al. Knowledge about hand shaping and knowledge about objects. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.
[30] Anna M. Borghi,et al. Conceptual information about size of objects in nouns , 2009 .
[31] Hisashi Q. Higuchi. On the nature of , 1999 .
[32] Kim Wilson,et al. The Benefits of Sensorimotor Knowledge: Body-Object Interaction Facilitates Semantic Processing , 2008, Cogn. Sci..
[33] M. Brodeur,et al. The Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS), a New Set of 480 Normative Photos of Objects to Be Used as Visual Stimuli in Cognitive Research , 2010, PloS one.
[34] D. Bub,et al. On the dynamics of action representations evoked by names of manipulable objects. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[35] Eva Belke. Long-lasting inhibitory semantic context effects on object naming are necessarily conceptually mediated: Implications for models of lexical-semantic encoding , 2013 .
[36] J. Haxby,et al. fMRI Responses to Video and Point-Light Displays of Moving Humans and Manipulable Objects , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[37] Target-related distractors disrupt object selection in everyday action: Evidence from participants with dementia , 2010, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[38] Michael Andres,et al. Dissociation between manipulation and conceptual knowledge of object use in the supramarginalis gyrus , 2011, Human brain mapping.
[39] G. Rizzolatti,et al. The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[40] L. Buxbaum,et al. Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and nonapraxic subjects , 2002, Brain and Language.
[41] Luciano Fadiga,et al. Hand action preparation influences the responses to hand pictures , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[42] Myrna F. Schwartz,et al. Function and manipulation tool knowledge in apraxia: Knowing ‘what for’ but not ‘how’ , 2000 .
[43] Julie C. Sedivy,et al. Playing on the typewriter, typing on the piano: manipulation knowledge of objects , 2006, Cognition.
[44] Tim Shallice,et al. Refractoriness and the healthy brain: A behavioural study on semantic access , 2011, Cognition.
[45] Markus Graf,et al. The role of action representations in visual object recognition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[46] L. Buxbaum,et al. Action matters: The role of action plans and object affordances in selection for action , 2002 .
[47] Stephen McAdams,et al. Comparison of Methods for Collecting and Modeling Dissimilarity Data: Applications to Complex Sound Stimuli , 2011, Multivariate behavioral research.
[48] Eva Belke,et al. Cumulative and Non-Cumulative Semantic Interference in Object Naming: Evidence from Blocked and Continuous Manipulations of Semantic Context , 2013, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[49] Chris McNorgan,et al. An attractor model of lexical conceptual processing: simulating semantic priming , 1999, Cogn. Sci..
[50] T. Shallice,et al. Category specific semantic impairments , 1984 .
[51] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[52] D. Mirman,et al. Abnormal dynamics of activation of object use information in apraxia: Evidence from eyetracking , 2014, Neuropsychologia.
[53] David P Vinson,et al. Semantic feature production norms for a large set of objects and events , 2008, Behavior research methods.
[54] M. Kiefer,et al. Action observation can prime visual object recognition , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[55] Gary M. Oppenheim,et al. The dark side of incremental learning: A model of cumulative semantic interference during lexical access in speech production , 2010, Cognition.
[56] Bradford Z. Mahon,et al. Lexical selection is not by competition: a reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[57] S. Thompson-Schill,et al. Function follows form: activation of shape and function features during object identification. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[58] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things , 2005, Behavior research methods.
[59] Olaf B. Paulson,et al. When Action Turns into Words. Activation of Motor-Based Knowledge during Categorization of Manipulable Objects , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[60] Harold Bekkering,et al. Body schematics: On the role of the body schema in embodied lexical–semantic representations , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[61] M. Garrett,et al. Representing the meanings of object and action words: The featural and unitary semantic space hypothesis , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.
[62] Susan T. Dumais,et al. A solution to Plato''s problem: representation of knowledge , 1997 .
[63] A. Meyer,et al. Refractory effects in picture naming as assessed in a semantic blocking paradigm , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[64] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[65] B. Mesquita,et al. Adjustment to Chronic Diseases and Terminal Illness Health Psychology : Psychological Adjustment to Chronic Disease , 2006 .
[66] M. Brysbaert,et al. Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words , 2012, Behavior research methods.
[67] Robert Schreuder,et al. Effects of perceptual and conceptual similarity in semantic priming , 1984 .
[68] T. Shallice,et al. Manipulability and object recognition: is manipulability a semantic feature? , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[69] R. Ellis,et al. On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[70] M. Coltheart,et al. Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies , 2006, Cognition.
[71] Michael P. Kaschak,et al. Grounding language in action , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[72] L. Buxbaum,et al. Critical brain regions for tool-related and imitative actions: a componential analysis. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[73] D L Medin,et al. Concepts and conceptual structure. , 1989, The American psychologist.
[74] Michael Andres,et al. Distinct contribution of the parietal and temporal cortex to hand configuration and contextual judgements about tools , 2013, Cortex.
[75] J. Kroll,et al. Category Interference in Translation and Picture Naming: Evidence for Asymmetric Connections Between Bilingual Memory Representations , 1994 .
[76] Eiling Yee,et al. Colorless Green Ideas (Can) Prime Furiously , 2012, Psychological science.
[77] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action for perception: a motor-visual attentional effect. , 1999 .
[78] Daniel N Bub,et al. Features of Planned Hand Actions Influence Identification of Graspable Objects , 2013, Psychological science.
[79] Susan T. Dumais,et al. The latent semantic analysis theory of knowledge , 1997 .
[80] L. Barsalou,et al. Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[81] Steven A. Jax,et al. Response interference between functional and structural object-related actions is increased in patients with ideomotor apraxia. , 2013, Journal of neuropsychology.
[82] A. A. Wijers,et al. Visual semantic features are activated during the processing of concrete words: event-related potential evidence for perceptual semantic priming. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[83] M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al. Conceptual Structure within and between Modalities , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[84] R. Marshall. 5. Multidimensional Scaling. 2nd edn. Trevor F. Cox and Michael A. A. Cox, Chapman & Hall/CRC, Boca Raton, London, New York, Washington DC, 2000. No. of pages: xiv + 309. Price: $79.95. ISBN 1‐58488‐094‐5 , 2002 .
[85] F. Pulvermüller,et al. Words in the brain's language , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[86] Patrick J. F. Groenen,et al. Modern Multidimensional Scaling: Theory and Applications , 2003 .
[87] Solène Kalénine,et al. Object manipulability affects children’s and adults’ conceptual processing , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[88] Murray Grossman,et al. Left Inferior Parietal Representations for Skilled Hand-Object Interactions: Evidence from Stroke and Corticobasal Degeneration , 2007, Cortex.
[89] Steven A. Jax,et al. Response interference between functional and structural actions linked to the same familiar object , 2010, Cognition.
[90] Lauren M. Bylsma,et al. Toward an integrated account of object and action selection: A computational analysis and empirical findings from reaching-to-grasp and tool-use , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[91] J. Haxby,et al. Parallel Visual Motion Processing Streams for Manipulable Objects and Human Movements , 2002, Neuron.
[92] L. Barsalou. Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.
[93] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Language Comprehenders Mentally Represent the Shapes of Objects , 2002, Psychological science.
[94] D. Mirman,et al. Incidental and context-responsive activation of structure- and function-based action features during object identification. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[95] R. Klatzky,et al. Cognitive representations of hand posture in ideomotor apraxia , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[96] Penny M. Pexman,et al. Evidence for the activation of sensorimotor information during visual word recognition: The body–object interaction effect , 2008, Cognition.
[97] Alex Martin,et al. Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.
[98] W. Schneider,et al. Perceptual Knowledge Retrieval Activates Sensory Brain Regions , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[99] Antje S. Meyer,et al. Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .
[100] D. Westwood,et al. Manipulability and living/non-living category effects on object identification , 2005, Brain and Cognition.
[101] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Premotor Cortex Activation during Observation and Naming of Familiar Tools , 1997, NeuroImage.
[102] Michael P. Kaschak,et al. The Action-Sentence Compatibility Effect: It's All in the Timing , 2006, Cogn. Sci..
[103] Randi C. Martin,et al. Consequences of an inhibition deficit for word production and comprehension: Evidence from the semantic blocking paradigm , 2008, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[104] Daniel N. Bub,et al. Language-Based Access to Gestural Components of Conceptual Knowledge , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[105] Matthew H. Davis,et al. Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time-course study , 2000 .