Intermittency in human manual tracking tasks.

We confirm Craik's (1947) observation that the human manual1y tracking a visual target behaves like an intermittent servo-control1er. Such tracking responses are indicative of "sampled" negative-feedback control but could be the result of other, continuous, mechanisms. Tracking performance therefore was recorded in a task in which visual feedback of the position of the hand-held joystick could be eliminated. Depriving the subjects of visual feedback led to smoother tracking and greatly reduced the signal power of their responses between 0.5-1.8 Hz. Their responses remained intermittent when they used feedback of their own position but not of the target to track a remembered (virtual) target. Hence, intermittency in tracking behavior is not exclusively a signature of visual feedback control but also may be a sign of feedback to memorized waveforms. Craik's (1947) suggestion that the intermittency is due to a refractory period following each movement was also tested. The errors measured at the start of each intermittent response, during tracking of slow waveforms, showed evidence of a small error deadzone (measuring 0.7 cm on the VDU screen or 0.80 degrees at the eye). At higher target speeds, however, the mean size of starting errors increased, and the upper boundary of the distribution of starting error was close to that expected of a refractory delay of approximately 170 ms between responses. We consider a model of the control system that can fit these results by incorporating an error deadzone within a feedback control loop. We therefore propose that the initiation of intermittent tracking responses may be limited by a positional error deadzone and that evidence for a refractory period between successive corrective movements can be satisfied without evoking an explicit timing or sampling mechanism.

[1]  K. J. Craik THEORY OF THE HUMAN OPERATOR IN CONTROL SYSTEMS , 1948 .

[2]  M. Vince The intermittency of control movements and the psychological refractory period. , 1948, The British journal of psychology. General section.

[3]  E. C. Poulton,et al.  THE BASIS OF PERCEPTUAL ANTICIPATION IN TRACKING , 1952 .

[4]  O Smith,et al.  CLOSER CONTROL OF LOOPS WITH DEAD TIME , 1957 .

[5]  George A. Bekey,et al.  The Human Operator as a Sampled-Data System , 1962 .

[6]  R. W. Pew,et al.  Performance of Human Operators in a Three-State Relay Control System With Velocity-Augmented Displays , 1966 .

[7]  R. W. Pew,et al.  Sine-Wave Tracking Revisited , 1967 .

[8]  M. C. Smith,et al.  Theories of the psychological refractory period. , 1967, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  M. Posner,et al.  Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  L. Stark,et al.  Sampling or intermittency in hand control system dynamics. , 1968, Biophysical journal.

[11]  Lawrence Stark,et al.  Neurological Control Systems: Studies in Bioengineering , 1995 .

[12]  C. I. Howarth,et al.  Movement Control in a Repetitive Motor Task , 1970, Nature.

[13]  Harvey M. Sussman,et al.  Delayed feedback in steering during learning and transfer of learning. , 1970 .

[14]  R W Angel,et al.  Correction of tracking errors without sensory feedback. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.

[15]  R. W. Pew,et al.  Human perceptual-motor performance , 1974 .

[16]  E. C. Poulton,et al.  Tracking skill and manual control , 1974 .

[17]  R.Wade Allen,et al.  The man/machine control interface - Pursuit control , 1979, Autom..

[18]  W. M. Smith,et al.  The effects of delayed and displaced visual feedback on motor control. , 1980, Journal of motor behavior.

[19]  Duane T. McRuer,et al.  Human dynamics in man-machine systems , 1980, Autom..

[20]  Lorraine G. Kisselburgh,et al.  Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  H. Beppu,et al.  Analysis of cerebellar motor disorders by visually guided elbow tracking movement. , 1984, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  Ernest O. Doebelin Control System Principles and Design , 1985 .

[23]  R. Miall,et al.  Visuomotor tracking with delayed visual feedback , 1985, Neuroscience.

[24]  R. Miall,et al.  Manual tracking of visual targets by trained monkeys , 1986, Behavioural Brain Research.

[25]  R. Jagacinski,et al.  Describing movement control at two levels of abstraction , 1987 .

[26]  M. Nagaoka,et al.  Analysis of cerebellar motor disorders by visually-guided elbow tracking movement. 2. Contribution of the visual cues on slow ramp pursuit. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  D. J. Weir,et al.  Planning of movement parameters in a visuo-motor tracking task , 1988, Behavioural Brain Research.

[28]  R. Miall,et al.  Cues and control strategies in visually guided tracking. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  L Glass,et al.  Complex oscillations in a human motor system. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[30]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Evidence for an error deadzone in compensatory tracking. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[31]  Lawrence A. Stark,et al.  Neurological Control Systems: Studies in Bioengineering , 1995 .