What is simulated in the action observation network when we observe actions?

In the last decade there has been a great amount of research investigating the role of simulation in our ability to infer the underlying intentions of any observed action. The majority of studies have focussed on the role of mirror neurons and the network of cortical areas active during action observation (AON) in inferring the goal of an observed action. However, it remains unclear what precisely is simulated when we observe an action and how such simulations can enable the observer to infer the underlying intention of that action. In particular it is not known how simulation in the AON enables the inference of the same goal when the kinematics observed to achieve that goal differ, such as when reaching to grasp an object with the left or right hands. Here we performed a behavioural study with healthy human subjects to address this question. We show that the subjects were able to detect very subtle changes in the kinematics of an observed action. In addition, we fitted the behavioural responses with a model based on the predictive coding account of mirror neurons. This is a Bayesian account of action observation that can be explained by the free‐energy principle. Here we show that we can model all the effects observed when the action observation system is considered within a predictive coding framework.

[1]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The mirror-neuron system: a Bayesian perspective. , 2007, Neuroreport.

[2]  Karl J. Friston The free-energy principle: a rough guide to the brain? , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Action generation and action perception in imitation: an instance of the ideomotor principle. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[4]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  James Gordon,et al.  Manual asymmetries in grasp pre-shaping and transport–grasp coordination , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  H. Ehrsson,et al.  Behavioural Brain Research , 1999 .

[7]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding under the free-energy principle , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The motor theory of social cognition: a critique , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  How does the mirror neuron system change during development? , 2007, Developmental science.

[10]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[11]  R. Dolan,et al.  Threatening a rubber hand that you feel is yours elicits a cortical anxiety response , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Scott T. Grafton Embodied Cognition and the Simulation of Action to Understand Others , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[13]  R. Passingham,et al.  That's My Hand! Activity in Premotor Cortex Reflects Feeling of Ownership of a Limb , 2004, Science.

[14]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[15]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A free energy principle for the brain , 2006, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[16]  W. Prinz Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .

[17]  A. Goldman,et al.  Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  M. Iacoboni Neural mechanisms of imitation , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[20]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system , 2007, Cognitive Processing.

[21]  Afdc Hamilton,et al.  The motor hierarchy: from kinematics to goals and intentions , 2007 .

[22]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.

[23]  Karl J. Friston Learning and inference in the brain , 2003, Neural Networks.

[24]  R. Saxe Against simulation: the argument from error , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  A unifying view of the basis of social cognition , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Touching a Rubber Hand: Feeling of Body Ownership Is Associated with Activity in Multisensory Brain Areas , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  S. Gallagher Simulation trouble , 2007, Social neuroscience.