Effects of hippocampal cholinergic deafferentation on learning strategy selection in a visible platform version of the water maze

Recent evidence has suggested that the relative levels of acetylcholine (ACh) between brain structures may be an important factor in the choice of behavioral strategy in settings in which either hippocampal or dorsal striatal brain systems can be employed both effectively and independently (McIntyre and Gold. 1999. Soc Neurosci Abs 25:1388). The current investigation used the neurotoxin 192 IgG‐saporin to deplete the hippocampus of ACh selectively, while leaving ACh in other brain regions, including dorsal striatum, intact. Rats were then trained on a version of the Morris water maze, in which behavioral strategies attributed to the hippocampus and dorsal striatum are placed in direct competition. It was predicted that rats with hippocampal ACh depletion would display a cue bias. Contrary to this prediction, depleting hippocampal ACh did not bias against and, in fact, promoted use of a hippocampal place strategy in this task, as indicated by choice in competition tests and performance on hidden platform training trials. These data add to a growing literature demonstrating that the septohippocampal cholinergic system is not required for accurate spatial learning and suggest a complex role for basal forebrain projections in processing information about the spatial environment. © 2003 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  P. E. Gold,et al.  Competition between Memory Systems: Acetylcholine Release in the Hippocampus Correlates Negatively with Good Performance on an Amygdala-Dependent Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  S. Mizumori,et al.  Dorsal striatal head direction and hippocampal place representations during spatial navigation , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  M. Gallagher,et al.  Intact spatial learning following lesions of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[4]  L. Squire,et al.  Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. , 1980, Science.

[5]  Differential effects of selective immunotoxic lesions of medial septal cholinergic cells on spatial working and reference memory. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[6]  R. Morris,et al.  Place navigation in rats is impaired by lesions of medial septum and diagonal band but not nucleus basalis magnocellularis , 1988, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  N. White,et al.  Parallel Information Processing in the Dorsal Striatum: Relation to Hippocampal Function , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  J. Coyle,et al.  Alzheimer's disease and senile dementia: loss of neurons in the basal forebrain. , 1982, Science.

[9]  K. Pang,et al.  GABAergic septohippocampal neurons are not necessary for spatial memory , 2001, Hippocampus.

[10]  K. Pang,et al.  Interactions between 192-IgG saporin and intraseptal cholinergic and GABAergic drugs: role of cholinergic medial septal neurons in spatial working memory. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  M. L. Smith,et al.  Cholinergic and gabaergic neurons in the nucleus basalis region of young and aged rats , 1995, Neuroscience.

[12]  S. Mizumori,et al.  Excitotoxic Septal Lesions Result in Spatial Memory Deficits and Altered Flexibility of Hippocampal Single-Unit Representations , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  J. Stemmelin,et al.  Immunohistochemical and neurochemical correlates of learning deficits in aged rats , 2000, Neuroscience.

[14]  T. Bowman,et al.  Transverse Patterning Reveals a Dissociation of Simple and Configural Association Learning Abilities in Rats with 192 IgG-Saporin Lesions of the Nucleus Basalis Magnocellularis , 2002, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[15]  M. Baxter,et al.  Cholinergic and noncholinergic septal neurons modulate strategy selection in spatial learning , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[16]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Laminar selectivity of the cholinergic suppression of synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal region CA1: computational modeling and brain slice physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Cholinergic and non-cholinergic septohippocampal pathways , 1985, Neuroscience Letters.

[18]  M. Baxter,et al.  Cholinergic basal forebrain is critical for social transmission of food preferences , 2000, Hippocampus.

[19]  M. Gallagher,et al.  Evaluation of Muscarinic M2 Receptor Sites in Basal Forebrain and Brainstem Cholinergic Systems of Behaviorally Characterized Young and Aged Long-Evans Rats , 1998, Neurobiology of Aging.

[20]  M. Baxter,et al.  Selective immunolesions of hippocampal cholinergic input fail to impair spatial working memory , 1997, Hippocampus.

[21]  S. Heckers,et al.  Differential effects on spatial navigation of immunotoxin-induced cholinergic lesions of the medial septal area and nucleus basalis magnocellularis , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Double dissociation of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on acquisition of two water maze tasks: further evidence for multiple memory systems. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[23]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Free recall and recognition in a network model of the hippocampus: simulating effects of scopolamine on human memory function , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Inactivation of Hippocampus or Caudate Nucleus with Lidocaine Differentially Affects Expression of Place and Response Learning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[25]  J. Morrison,et al.  Circuit-Specific Alterations in Hippocampal Synaptophysin Immunoreactivity Predict Spatial Learning Impairment in Aged Rats , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Karnovsky,et al.  A "DIRECT-COLORING" THIOCHOLINE METHOD FOR CHOLINESTERASES , 1964, The journal of histochemistry and cytochemistry : official journal of the Histochemistry Society.

[27]  U. Greferath,et al.  Impaired spatial learning in aged rats is associated with loss of p75-positive neurons in the basal forebrain , 2000, Neuroscience.

[28]  T. Freund,et al.  Distribution of septohippocampal neurons containing parvalbumin or choline acetyltransferase in the rat brain , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[29]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Dynamics of learning and recall at excitatory recurrent synapses and cholinergic modulation in rat hippocampal region CA3 , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[30]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[31]  Tamás F. Freund,et al.  GABAergic septohippocampal neurons contain parvalbumin , 1989, Brain Research.

[32]  G. Paxinos,et al.  The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates , 1983 .

[33]  C. Geula,et al.  Complete and selective cholinergic denervation of rat neocortex and hippocampus but not amygdala by an immunotoxin against the p75 NGF receptor , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  R. Bartus,et al.  The cholinergic hypothesis of geriatric memory dysfunction. , 1982, Science.

[35]  M. Gallagher,et al.  Decreased glucocorticoid receptor mRNA and dysfunction of HPA axis in rats after removal of the cholinergic innervation to hippocampus. , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.

[36]  M. M. Glasier,et al.  Intraseptal injections of 192 IgG saporin produce deficits for strategy selection in spatial-memory tasks , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.

[37]  M. Gallagher,et al.  Markers for biogenic amines in the aged rat brain: Relationship to decline in spatial learning ability , 1990, Neurobiology of Aging.

[38]  T. J. Walsh,et al.  Injection of IgG 192-saporin into the medial septum produces cholinergic hypofunction and dose-dependent working memory deficits , 1996, Brain Research.

[39]  M. Laakso,et al.  Neocortical, hippocampal and septal parvalbumin- and somatostatin-containing neurons in young and aged rats: Correlation with passive avoidance and water maze performance , 1993, Neuroscience.

[40]  M. Gallagher,et al.  Impairments in conditioned stimulus processing and conditioned responding after combined selective removal of hippocampal and neocortical cholinergic input. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  A. Chiba,et al.  A re-examination of the role of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in spatial working memory , 1998, Neuropharmacology.

[42]  M. Gallagher,et al.  The use of animal models to study the effects of aging on cognition. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[43]  F. Restle Discrimination of cues in mazes: a resolution of the place-vs.-response question. , 1957, Psychological review.

[44]  Janis Ls,et al.  Medial septal lesions in rats produce permanent deficits for strategy selection in a spatial memory task. , 1994 .

[45]  V. Chan‐Palay,et al.  Septal neurons containing glutamic acid decarboxylase immunoreactivity project to the hippocampal region in the rat brain , 2004, Anatomy and Embryology.

[46]  L. Butcher,et al.  Cholinergic systems in the rat brain: I. Projections to the limbic telencephalon , 1984, Brain Research Bulletin.

[47]  B. Knowlton,et al.  Learning and memory functions of the Basal Ganglia. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[48]  M. Mishkin,et al.  The neuroanatomy of amnesia: amygdala-hippocampus versus temporal stem. , 1982, Science.

[49]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  Parallel information processing in the water maze: evidence for independent memory systems involving dorsal striatum and hippocampus. , 1994, Behavioral and neural biology.

[50]  C. Barnes Memory deficits associated with senescence: a neurophysiological and behavioral study in the rat. , 1979, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[51]  M. Packard,et al.  Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[52]  M. Gallagher,et al.  Intact spatial learning in both young and aged rats following selective removal of hippocampal cholinergic input. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.