A Representation of Effort in Decision-Making and Motor Control

[1]  J. Emlen The Role of Time and Energy in Food Preference , 1966, The American Naturalist.

[2]  M J Kushmerick,et al.  Aerobic recovery metabolism following a single isometric tetanus in frog sartorius muscle at 0 degrees C. , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  D. F. Hoyt,et al.  Gait and the energetics of locomotion in horses , 1981, Nature.

[4]  A G Barto,et al.  Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction. , 1981, Psychological review.

[5]  G. Brooks,et al.  Exercise physiology: Human bioenergetics and its applications , 1984 .

[6]  J. Brockway Derivation of formulae used to calculate energy expenditure in man. , 1987, Human nutrition. Clinical nutrition.

[7]  J. Sorkin,et al.  Fasting respiratory exchange ratio and resting metabolic rate as predictors of weight gain: the Baltimore Longitudinal Study on Aging. , 1992, International journal of obesity and related metabolic disorders : journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity.

[8]  L. Green,et al.  Discounting of delayed rewards: Models of individual choice. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[9]  K. Short,et al.  Excess postexercise oxygen consumption and recovery rate in trained and untrained subjects. , 1997, Journal of applied physiology.

[10]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Expectation of reward modulates cognitive signals in the basal ganglia , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  R. Levine,et al.  The Pace of Life in 31 Countries , 1999 .

[12]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Modulation of saccadic eye movements by predicted reward outcome , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  To walk or to fly? How birds choose among foraging modes. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  J. Gold,et al.  Banburismus and the Brain Decoding the Relationship between Sensory Stimuli, Decisions, and Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[15]  J. Salamone,et al.  Nucleus accumbens dopamine and work requirements on interval schedules , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[16]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  Krista Vandenborne,et al.  Metabolic costs of isometric force generation and maintenance of human skeletal muscle. , 2002, American journal of physiology. Endocrinology and metabolism.

[18]  H. Sugiwaka,et al.  Reformative self-control and discounting of reward value by delay or effort1 , 2004 .

[19]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  A Neuroeconomics Approach to Inferring Utility Functions in Sensorimotor Control , 2004, PLoS biology.

[20]  J. Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  H. Ralston Energy-speed relation and optimal speed during level walking , 1958, Internationale Zeitschrift für angewandte Physiologie einschließlich Arbeitsphysiologie.

[22]  P. Willems,et al.  Effect of load and speed on the energetic cost of human walking , 2005, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[23]  J. Salamone,et al.  Haloperidol and nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion suppress lever pressing for food but increase free food consumption in a novel food choice procedure , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[24]  Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al.  Weighing up the benefits of work: Behavioral and neural analyses of effort-related decision making , 2006, Neural Networks.

[25]  Kenji Doya,et al.  Humans Can Adopt Optimal Discounting Strategy under Real-Time Constraints , 2006, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[26]  P. Dayan,et al.  Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor , 2007, Psychopharmacology.

[27]  Natalia V Dounskaia,et al.  Directional biases reveal utilization of arm's biomechanical properties for optimization of motor behavior. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Motor Adaptation as a Process of Reoptimization , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  W. Schultz,et al.  Influence of Reward Delays on Responses of Dopamine Neurons , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  The intrinsic value of visual information affects saccade velocities , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Manoj Srinivasan,et al.  Optimal speeds for walking and running, and walking on a moving walkway. , 2009, Chaos.

[32]  Etienne Burdet,et al.  Dissociating Variability and Effort as Determinants of Coordination , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[33]  Koji Jimura,et al.  Are people really more patient than other animals? Evidence from human discounting of real liquid rewards , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[34]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Temporal Discounting of Reward and the Cost of Time in Motor Control , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Jeremy D Wong,et al.  The central nervous system does not minimize energy cost in arm movements. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  Mathias Pessiglione,et al.  Separate Valuation Subsystems for Delay and Effort Decision Costs , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Anatol C. Kreitzer,et al.  Regulation of parkinsonian motor behaviours by optogenetic control of basal ganglia circuitry , 2010, Nature.

[38]  R. Nelson,et al.  Motor Planning under Unpredictable Reward: Modulations of Movement Vigor and Primate Striatum Activity , 2011, Front. Neurosci..

[39]  P. Cisek,et al.  The influence of predicted arm biomechanics on decision making. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Natalia Dounskaia,et al.  Load emphasizes muscle effort minimization during selection of arm movement direction , 2012, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[41]  Thomas R. Reppert,et al.  Evidence for Hyperbolic Temporal Discounting of Reward in Control of Movements , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  Lionel Rigoux,et al.  A Model of Reward- and Effort-Based Optimal Decision Making and Motor Control , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[43]  Helen J. Huang,et al.  Reduction of Metabolic Cost during Motor Learning of Arm Reaching Dynamics , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  R. Turner,et al.  Limited Encoding of Effort by Dopamine Neurons in a Cost–Benefit Trade-off Task , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Eun Jung Hwang,et al.  The basal ganglia, the ideal machinery for the cost-benefit analysis of action plans , 2013, Front. Neural Circuits.

[46]  David A. Rosenbaum,et al.  What is chosen first, the hand used for reaching or the target that is reached? , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[47]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Context-Dependent Urgency Influences Speed–Accuracy Trade-Offs in Decision-Making and Movement Execution , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  Paul Cisek,et al.  On the challenges and mechanisms of embodied decisions , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[49]  Jennie E. S. Choi,et al.  Vigor of Movements and the Cost of Time in Decision Making , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Motor Costs and the Coordination of the Two Arms , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  William D. Penny,et al.  Behavioral Modeling of Human Choices Reveals Dissociable Effects of Physical Effort and Temporal Delay on Reward Devaluation , 2015, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[52]  M. Rice,et al.  Degree of Preference and Its Influence on Motor Control When Reaching for Most Preferred, Neutrally Preferred, and Least Preferred Candy , 2015, OTJR : occupation, participation and health.

[53]  Jessica C. Selinger,et al.  Humans Can Continuously Optimize Energetic Cost during Walking , 2015, Current Biology.

[54]  Karolina M. Lempert,et al.  Modulation of Saccade Vigor during Value-Based Decision Making , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  Rajiv Ranganathan,et al.  Sensory Agreement Guides Kinetic Energy Optimization of Arm Movements during Object Manipulation , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..