If so many are “few,” how few are “many”?

The scope of reference of a word's meaning can be highly variable. We present a novel paradigm to investigate the flexible interpretation of word meaning. We focus on quantifiers such as “many” or “few,” a class of words that depends on number knowledge but can be interpreted in a flexible manner. Healthy young adults performed a truth value judgment task on pictorial arrays of varying amounts of blue and yellow circles, deciding whether the sentence “Many/few of the circles are yellow” was an adequate description of the stimulus. The study consisted of two experiments, one focusing on “many,” one on “few.” Each experiment had three blocks. In a first “baseline” block, each individual's criterion for “many” and “few” was assessed. In a second “adaptation” block, subjects received feedback about their decisions that was different from their initial judgments in an effort to evaluate the flexibility of a subject's interpretation. A third “test” block assessed whether adaptation of quantifier meaning induced in block 2 then was generalized to alter a subject's baseline meaning for “many” and “few.” In Experiment 1, a proportion of yellow circles as small as 40% was reinforced as “many”; in Experiment 2, a proportion of yellow circles as large as 60% was reinforced as “few.” Subjects learned the new criterion for “many” in Experiment 1, which also affected their criterion for “few” although it had never been mentioned. Likewise, in Experiment 2, subjects changed their criterion for “few,” with a comparable effect on the criterion for “many” which was not mentioned. Thus, the meaning of relational quantifiers like “many” and “few” is flexible and can be adapted. Most importantly, adapting the criterion for one quantifier (e.g., “many”) also appeared to affect the reciprocal quantifier (in this case, “few”). Implications of this result for psychological interventions and for investigations of the neurobiology of the language-number interface are discussed.

[1]  Stefan Heim,et al.  Processing of Numerical and Proportional Quantifiers , 2015, Cogn. Sci..

[2]  M. Garraffa,et al.  Working Memory Mechanism in Proportional Quantifier Verification , 2014, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[3]  Tao Yang,et al.  Dissociated neural correlates of quantity processing of quantifiers, numbers, and numerosities , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[4]  J. Price,et al.  The role of feedback and dot presentation format in younger and older adults’ number estimation , 2014, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[5]  Jakub Szymanik,et al.  Working Memory Mechanism in Proportional Quantifier Verification , 2013, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.

[6]  Robin Clark,et al.  Converging Evidence for the Processing Costs Associated with Ambiguous Quantifier Comprehension , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[7]  M. Grossman,et al.  fMRI evidence for strategic decision-making during resolution of pronoun reference , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  The Language–Number Interface in the Brain: A Complex Parametric Study of Quantifiers and Quantities , 2011, Front. Evol. Neurosci..

[9]  S. Loewen The role of feedback , 2011 .

[10]  Brian Avants,et al.  Some is not enough: Quantifier comprehension in corticobasal syndrome and behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  David Barner,et al.  Number words, quantifiers, and principles of word learning. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[12]  Robin Clark,et al.  Magnitude and parity as complementary attributes of quantifier statements , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Manuela Piazza,et al.  How Humans Count: Numerosity and the Parietal Cortex , 2009, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[14]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Asymmetries in the Acquisition of Numbers and Quantifiers , 2006 .

[15]  Robin Clark,et al.  Neural basis for generalized quantifier comprehension , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Anthony J. Sanford,et al.  New Perspectives on the Expression of Quantity , 2003 .

[17]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[18]  Bart Geurts,et al.  Reasoning with quantifiers , 2003, Cognition.

[19]  E. Miller,et al.  Coding of Cognitive Magnitude Compressed Scaling of Numerical Information in the Primate Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[20]  Michael Haugh The intuitive basis of implicature: relevance theoretic implicitness versus Gricean implying , 2002 .

[21]  Maria Bannert,et al.  Managing Cognitive Load--Recent Trends in Cognitive Load Theory. Commentary. , 2002 .

[22]  G. Logan,et al.  Memory-based automaticity in the discrimination of visual numerosity. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  Keith J. Holyoak,et al.  Recognition confusions among quantifiers , 1978 .

[24]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  On the process of comparing sentences against pictures , 1972 .

[25]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Linguistic processes in deductive reasoning. , 1969 .

[26]  H. Helson Adaptation-level as a basis for a quantitative theory of frames of reference. , 1948, Psychological review.

[27]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[28]  N. Chater,et al.  Relative informativeness of quantifiers used in syllogistic reasoning , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[29]  David A. Routh,et al.  On Representations of Quantifiers , 1994, J. Semant..