Research report Voluntary control of postural equilibrium patterns

The ability to voluntarily transit from one whole-body movement to another is based on the multisensory integration of visual, vestibular, and somatosensory information. The role of functional sensory ranges and mechanical constraints on the ability to voluntarily transit between whole-body movements was studied by requiring subjects to switch from a head-fixed-to-surface to head-fixed-in-space postural pattern (and vice versa). The head-fixed-to-surface pattern required an erect stance characterized by an in-phase relationship between center of pressure (CoP) and platform motion. The head-fixed-in-space pattern required subjects to fix trunk–head position in-space while producing an anti-phase relationship between CoP and platform motion. The voluntary transition was performed with and without vision while standing on a surface oscillating in the anterior–posterior (A/P) direction. The support surface oscillated at five frequencies (0.2–1 Hz) with amplitude fixed at 15 cm. The voluntary transition was initiated with an auditory cue. The appropriate CoP-platform phase relationship for the two postural patterns was produced for all frequencies with and without vision. Upper-trunk kinematics revealed that subjects often failed to produce the head-fixed-to-surface pattern for frequencies ≥0.6 Hz, while producing the head-fixed-in-space pattern at all frequencies with vision. Without vision, neither pattern was produced consistently based on upper-trunk kinematics. These findings demonstrate separate control processes for upper- and lower-body motion and that functional sensory ranges and mechanical constraints can facilitate or inhibit voluntary production of whole-body movements based on these control processes. The results are discussed in reference to neurological substrates that may be involved in the planning and execution of motor set-switching. The experimental protocol we employ may also have application as a diagnostic tool for the evaluation of postural deficits. © 2003 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.

[1]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Head stabilization during various locomotor tasks in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Christopher L. Vaughan,et al.  Dynamics of human gait , 1992 .

[3]  J C Rothwell,et al.  The Bereitschaftspotential preceding simple foot movement and initiation of gait in Parkinson's disease , 1993, Neurology.

[4]  C J Winstein,et al.  Function of the 'direct' and 'indirect' pathways of the basal ganglia motor loop: evidence from reciprocal aiming movements in Parkinson's disease. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[5]  T Mergner,et al.  Vestibular-neck interaction and transformation of sensory coordinates. , 1997, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[6]  F. Horak,et al.  Parkinson’s disease impairs the ability to change set quickly , 2000, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[7]  Martin A. Giese,et al.  Frequency dependence of the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving visual environment: relative phase dynamics , 1994, Biological Cybernetics.

[8]  C. Gielen,et al.  Temporal stability of the action-perception cycle for postural control in a moving visual environment , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  F. Horak,et al.  Emergence of postural patterns as a function of vision and translation frequency. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  M Schieppati,et al.  Group II spindle afferent fibers in humans: their possible role in the reflex control of stance. , 1999, Progress in brain research.

[11]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Head stabilization during various locomotor tasks in humans , 1990, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  T A Stoffregen,et al.  The role of optical velocity in the control of stance , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  H C Diener,et al.  On the role of vestibular, visual and somatosensory information for dynamic postural control in humans. , 1988, Progress in brain research.

[14]  G. McCollum,et al.  Form and exploration of mechanical stability limits in erect stance. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[15]  F. Horak,et al.  Limited control strategies with the loss of vestibular function , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  P. Roland,et al.  Supplementary motor area and other cortical areas in organization of voluntary movements in man. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  H J Sagar,et al.  Alternating fluency in Parkinson's disease. An evaluation of the attentional control theory of cognitive impairment. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  E. Walsh,et al.  Standing man, slow rhythmic tilting, importance of vision. , 1973, Agressologie: revue internationale de physio-biologie et de pharmacologie appliquees aux effets de l'agression.

[19]  F B Horak,et al.  Hip sway associated with vestibulopathy. , 1994, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[20]  John T. McConville,et al.  INVESTIGATION OF INERTIAL PROPERTIES OF THE HUMAN BODY , 1975 .

[21]  Thomas Rosemeier,et al.  Interaction of vestibular, somatosensory and visual signals for postural control and motion perception under terrestrial and microgravity conditions—a conceptual model , 1998, Brain Research Reviews.

[22]  F. Horak,et al.  Central programming of postural movements: adaptation to altered support-surface configurations. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  B. Guschlbauer,et al.  The significance of proprioception on postural stabilization as assessed by ischemia , 1984, Brain Research.

[24]  F B Horak,et al.  Vestibular loss disrupts control of head and trunk on a sinusoidally moving platform. , 2001, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[25]  V. Dietz,et al.  Human stance on a sinusoidally translating platform: balance control by feedforward and feedback mechanisms , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  J. Kelso,et al.  To Switch or Not to Switch: Recruitment of Degrees of Freedom Stabilizes Biological Coordination. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[27]  F. Horak,et al.  Cerebellar control of postural scaling and central set in stance. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  A. Bronstein,et al.  Influence of action and expectation on visual control of posture. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[29]  J. Mink THE BASAL GANGLIA: FOCUSED SELECTION AND INHIBITION OF COMPETING MOTOR PROGRAMS , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[30]  J. Kelso,et al.  Nonequilibrium phase transitions in coordinated biological motion: critical fluctuations , 1986 .

[31]  J. Kelso,et al.  Intentional switching between patterns of bimanual coordination depends on the intrinsic dynamics of the patterns. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[32]  W. Bruzek,et al.  Stabilization of human posture during induced oscillations of the body , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  S. Keele,et al.  Toward a Functional Analysis of the Basal Ganglia , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Patricia S. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Toward a circuit model of working memory and the guidance of voluntary motor action , 1995 .

[35]  F B Horak,et al.  Control of stance during lateral and anterior/posterior surface translations. , 1998, IEEE transactions on rehabilitation engineering : a publication of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[36]  F. Horak,et al.  The importance of somatosensory information in triggering and scaling automatic postural responses in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  L. Nashner Adapting reflexes controlling the human posture , 1976, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Fay B. Horak,et al.  Time-dependent influence of sensorimotor set on automatic responses in perturbed stance , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  Meir Plotnik,et al.  Mental and Motor Switching in Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[40]  Fay B. Horak,et al.  Postural Adaptation for Altered Environments, Tasks, and Intentions , 2000 .

[41]  F. O. Black,et al.  Organization of posture controls: an analysis of sensory and mechanical constraints. , 1989, Progress in brain research.

[42]  Antonio Nardone,et al.  Standing on a continuously moving platform: is body inertia counteracted or exploited? , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  F B Horak,et al.  Centrally initiated postural adjustments in parkinsonian patients on and off levodopa. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  V. Gurfinkel,et al.  Postural reactions to the controlled sinusoidal displacement of the supporting platform. , 1976, Agressologie: revue internationale de physio-biologie et de pharmacologie appliquees aux effets de l'agression.

[45]  Fay B. Horak,et al.  Transitions in a postural task: do the recruitment and suppression of degrees of freedom stabilize posture? , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  F. Horak,et al.  Influence of instruction, prediction, and afferent sensory information on the postural organization of step initiation. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.