Two Spatially Distinct Posterior Alpha Sources Fulfill Different Functional Roles in Attention

Directing attention helps to extract relevant information and suppress distracters. Alpha brain oscillations (8-12Hz) play a fundamental role in this process, with a power decrease facilitating processing of important information and power increase inhibiting brain regions processing irrelevant information. Evidence for this phenomenon arises from visual attention studies (Worden et al., 2000), however, the effect also exists in other modalities, including the somatosensory system (Haegens et al., 2011) and inter-sensory attention tasks (Foxe and Snyder, 2011). We investigated what happens when attention is divided between two modalities using both a multi- and unimodal attention paradigm while recording EEG over 128 scalp electrodes in two separate experiments. In Experiment 1 participants divided their attention between the visual and somatosensory modality to determine the temporal or spatial frequency of a target stimulus (vibrotactile stimulus or Gabor grating). In Experiment 2, participants divided attention between two visual hemifields to identify the orientation of a target Gabor grating. In both experiments, pre-stimulus alpha power in visual areas decreased linearly with increasing attention to visual stimuli. In contrast, alpha power in parietal areas showed lower pre-stimulus alpha power when attention was divided between modalities, compared to unimodal attention. These results suggest that there are two different alpha sources, where one reflects the ‘visual spotlight of attention’ and the other reflects attentional effort. To our knowledge, this is the first study to show that attention recruits two spatially distinct alpha sources in occipital and parietal brain regions, which act simultaneously but serve different functions in attention. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Attention to one spatial location/sensory modality leads to power changes of alpha oscillations (~10Hz) with decreased power over regions processing relevant information and power increases to actively inhibit areas processing ‘to-be-ignored’ information. Here, we used detailed source modelling to investigate EEG data recorded during separate uni-modal (visual) and multi- (visual and somatosensory) attention tasks. Participants either focused their attention on one modality/spatial location or directed it to both. We show for the first time two distinct alpha sources are active simultaneously but play different roles. A sensory (visual) alpha source was linearly modulated by attention representing the ‘visual spotlight of attention’. In contrast, a parietal alpha source was modulated by attentional effort, showing lowest alpha power when attention was divided.

[1]  F. Crick Function of the thalamic reticular complex: the searchlight hypothesis. , 1984, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  J. Palva,et al.  New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[3]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Frontoparietal Cortex Controls Spatial Attention through Modulation of Anticipatory Alpha Rhythms , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  E. Halgren,et al.  The generation and propagation of the human alpha rhythm , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  G. V. Simpson,et al.  Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific α-Bank Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Oscillatory Sensory Selection Mechanisms during Intersensory Attention to Rhythmic Auditory and Visual Inputs: A Human Electrocorticographic Investigation , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  M. Ding,et al.  Attentional modulation of the somatosensory mu rhythm , 2011, Neuroscience.

[8]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Resampling-based false discovery rate controlling multiple test procedures for correlated test statistics , 1999 .

[9]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The Role of Alpha-Band Brain Oscillations as a Sensory Suppression Mechanism during Selective Attention , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[10]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Event-related synchronization (ERS) in the alpha band--an electrophysiological correlate of cortical idling: a review. , 1996, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[11]  D. Stuss,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience. , 1993, Current opinion in neurobiology.

[12]  S. Hanslmayr,et al.  Episodic Memory Retrieval Functionally Relies on Very Rapid Reactivation of Sensory Information , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Ole Jensen,et al.  Frontal Eye Fields Control Attentional Modulation of Alpha and Gamma Oscillations in Contralateral Occipitoparietal Cortex , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Asymmetry of Anticipatory Activity in Visual Cortex Predicts the Locus of Attention and Perception , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  O. Jensen,et al.  Shaping Functional Architecture by Oscillatory Alpha Activity: Gating by Inhibition , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[16]  S. Kastner,et al.  FEF-Controlled Alpha Delay Activity Precedes Stimulus-Induced Gamma-Band Activity in Visual Cortex , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[18]  Stephen D. Mayhew,et al.  Addressing challenges of high spatial resolution, UHF field fMRI for group analysis of higher-order cognitive tasks; an inter-sensory task directing attention between visual and somatosensory domains , 2018 .

[19]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .

[20]  Guillaume A. Rousselet,et al.  Modeling Single-Trial ERP Reveals Modulation of Bottom-Up Face Visual Processing by Top-Down Task Constraints (in Some Subjects) , 2011, Frontiers in psychology.

[21]  R. VanRullen,et al.  An oscillatory mechanism for prioritizing salient unattended stimuli , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  A. Nobre,et al.  Indexing the graded allocation of visuospatial attention using anticipatory alpha oscillations , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Increases in alpha oscillatory power reflect an active retinotopic mechanism for distracter suppression during sustained visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  K. Mathewson,et al.  Pulsed Out of Awareness: EEG Alpha Oscillations Represent a Pulsed-Inhibition of Ongoing Cortical Processing , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[25]  H. Akaike A new look at the statistical model identification , 1974 .

[26]  Matthew J. Brookes,et al.  A multi-layer network approach to MEG connectivity analysis , 2016, NeuroImage.

[27]  W. Drongelen,et al.  A spatial filtering technique to detect and localize multiple sources in the brain , 1996, Brain Topography.

[28]  W. Klimesch,et al.  EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis , 2007, Brain Research Reviews.

[29]  Se Robinson,et al.  Functional neuroimaging by Synthetic Aperture Magnetometry (SAM) , 1999 .

[30]  P. Roelfsema,et al.  Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Top-down flow of visual spatial attention signals from parietal to occipital cortex. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[33]  Saskia Haegens,et al.  Somatosensory Anticipatory Alpha Activity Increases to Suppress Distracting Input , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Prestimulus Oscillatory Activity in the Alpha Band Predicts Visual Discrimination Ability , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Simon Hanslmayr,et al.  Addressing challenges of high spatial resolution UHF fMRI for group analysis of higher‐order cognitive tasks: An inter‐sensory task directing attention between visual and somatosensory domains , 2019, Human brain mapping.

[36]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[37]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Top-Down Control of Human Visual Cortex by Frontal and Parietal Cortex in Anticipatory Visual Spatial Attention , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  P. König,et al.  Top-down processing mediated by interareal synchronization. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Neuronal Dynamics Underlying High- and Low-Frequency EEG Oscillations Contribute Independently to the Human BOLD Signal , 2011, Neuron.

[40]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[41]  H H Donaldson,et al.  LOCALIZATION IN THE BRAIN. , 1884, Science.

[42]  R. Desimone,et al.  Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  E. Maris,et al.  Orienting Attention to an Upcoming Tactile Event Involves a Spatially and Temporally Specific Modulation of Sensorimotor Alpha- and Beta-Band Oscillations , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  O. Andreassen,et al.  Mice Deficient in Cellular Glutathione Peroxidase Show Increased Vulnerability to Malonate, 3-Nitropropionic Acid, and 1-Methyl-4-Phenyl-1,2,5,6-Tetrahydropyridine , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[46]  P. Nunez,et al.  Spatial‐temporal structures of human alpha rhythms: Theory, microcurrent sources, multiscale measurements, and global binding of local networks , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[47]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  University of Birmingham Occipital alpha activity during stimulus processing gates the information flow to object-selective cortex , 2014 .

[48]  W. Drongelen,et al.  Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering , 1997, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[49]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[50]  Barbara F. Händel,et al.  Top-Down Controlled Alpha Band Activity in Somatosensory Areas Determines Behavioral Performance in a Discrimination Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Helmut Laufs,et al.  Where the BOLD signal goes when alpha EEG leaves , 2006, NeuroImage.

[52]  Richard M. Leahy,et al.  Brainstorm: A User-Friendly Application for MEG/EEG Analysis , 2011, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[53]  Mark S. Cohen,et al.  Simultaneous EEG and fMRI of the alpha rhythm , 2002, Neuroreport.

[54]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Allocation of attention in the visual field. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  J. Driver,et al.  Attentional selection of location and modality in vision and touch modulates low-frequency activity in associated sensory cortices , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Distinct causal influences of parietal versus frontal areas on human visual cortex: evidence from concurrent TMS-fMRI. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[57]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  An Information-Maximization Approach to Blind Separation and Blind Deconvolution , 1995, Neural Computation.

[58]  G. V. Simpson,et al.  Parieto‐occipital ∼1 0Hz activity reflects anticipatory state of visual attention mechanisms , 1998 .

[59]  R. VanRullen,et al.  The Triple-Flash Illusion Reveals a Driving Role of Alpha-Band Reverberations in Visual Perception , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.