The brain uses efference copy information to optimise spatial memory

Does a motor response to a target improve the subsequent recall of the target position or can we simply use peripheral position information to guide an accurate response? We suggest that a motor plan of the hand can be enhanced with actual motor and efference copy feedback (GoGo trials), which is absent in the passive observation of a stimulus (NoGo trials). To investigate this effect during eye and hand coordination movements, we presented stimuli in two formats (memory guided or visually guided) under three modality conditions (eyes only, hands only (with eyes fixated), or eyes and hand together). We found that during coordinated movements, both the eye and hand response times were facilitated when efference feedback of the movement was provided. Furthermore, both eye and hand movements to remembered locations were significantly more accurate in the GoGo than in the NoGo trial types. These results reveal that an efference copy of a motor plan enhances memory for a location that is not only observed in eye movements, but also translated downstream into a hand movement. These results have significant implications on how we plan, code and guide behavioural responses, and how we can optimise accuracy and timing to a given target.

[1]  J. Wann,et al.  How active gaze informs the hand in sequential pointing movements , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  The Duncker illusion and eye-hand coordination. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Ocular perturbations and retinal/extraretinal information: the coordination of saccadic and manual movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  Eileen Kowler Cognitive expectations, not habits, control anticipatory smooth oculomotor pursuit , 1989, Vision Research.

[5]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  H Bekkering,et al.  Coordinated control of eye and hand movements in dynamic reaching. , 2002, Human movement science.

[7]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. , 1969, Vision research.

[8]  M. Biscaldi,et al.  The initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements and saccades in normal subjects and in “express-saccade makers” , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  G. R. Barnes,et al.  Anticipatory eye movements evoked after active following versus passive observation of a predictable motion stimulus , 2008, Brain Research.

[10]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[11]  O. Bock,et al.  Contribution of retinal versus extraretinal signals towards visual localization in goal-directed movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  Tal Makovski,et al.  Investigating the Role of Response in Spatial Context Learning , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  Paul van Donkelaar,et al.  Eye-hand coordination to visual versus remembered targets , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  R. Johansson,et al.  Gaze behavior when reaching to remembered targets. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Howard Poizner,et al.  The interaction of visual and proprioceptive inputs in pointing to actual and remembered targets , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  J D Crawford,et al.  Spatial transformations for eye-hand coordination. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Effect of retinal and/or extra-retinal information on age in memory-guided saccades , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Ruth Maulucci,et al.  Visually triggered eye-arm movements in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  E. Brenner,et al.  Combining eye and hand in search is suboptimal , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  L F Dell'Osso,et al.  Saccades to remembered targets: the effects of smooth pursuit and illusory stimulus motion. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  J D Crawford,et al.  Proprioceptive guidance of saccades in eye-hand coordination. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Lamarre,et al.  Self-moved target eye tracking in control and deafferented subjects: roles of arm motor command and proprioception in arm-eye coordination. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  M G Saslow,et al.  Latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[24]  Denise Y P Henriques,et al.  Reference frame conversions for repeated arm movements. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  R. Johansson,et al.  Eye–Hand Coordination in Object Manipulation , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Why Does the Brain Predict Sensory Consequences of Oculomotor Commands? Optimal Integration of the Predicted and the Actual Sensory Feedback , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  D. Meyer,et al.  Eye-hand coordination: oculomotor control in rapid aimed limb movements. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  Jos J. Adam,et al.  Reaction time latencies of eye and hand movements in single- and dual-task conditions , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.