The brain uses efference copy information to optimise spatial memory
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Wann,et al. How active gaze informs the hand in sequential pointing movements , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[2] J. F. Soechting,et al. The Duncker illusion and eye-hand coordination. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[3] Digby Elliott,et al. Ocular perturbations and retinal/extraretinal information: the coordination of saccadic and manual movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[4] Eileen Kowler. Cognitive expectations, not habits, control anticipatory smooth oculomotor pursuit , 1989, Vision Research.
[5] R. Klein,et al. Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[6] H Bekkering,et al. Coordinated control of eye and hand movements in dynamic reaching. , 2002, Human movement science.
[7] A. Fuchs,et al. Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. , 1969, Vision research.
[8] M. Biscaldi,et al. The initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements and saccades in normal subjects and in “express-saccade makers” , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] G. R. Barnes,et al. Anticipatory eye movements evoked after active following versus passive observation of a predictable motion stimulus , 2008, Brain Research.
[10] C. Prablanc,et al. Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.
[11] O. Bock,et al. Contribution of retinal versus extraretinal signals towards visual localization in goal-directed movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[12] Tal Makovski,et al. Investigating the Role of Response in Spatial Context Learning , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[13] Paul van Donkelaar,et al. Eye-hand coordination to visual versus remembered targets , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[14] R. Johansson,et al. Gaze behavior when reaching to remembered targets. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[15] Howard Poizner,et al. The interaction of visual and proprioceptive inputs in pointing to actual and remembered targets , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[16] J D Crawford,et al. Spatial transformations for eye-hand coordination. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[17] Effect of retinal and/or extra-retinal information on age in memory-guided saccades , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[18] Ruth Maulucci,et al. Visually triggered eye-arm movements in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] E. Brenner,et al. Combining eye and hand in search is suboptimal , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] L F Dell'Osso,et al. Saccades to remembered targets: the effects of smooth pursuit and illusory stimulus motion. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[21] J D Crawford,et al. Proprioceptive guidance of saccades in eye-hand coordination. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[22] Lamarre,et al. Self-moved target eye tracking in control and deafferented subjects: roles of arm motor command and proprioception in arm-eye coordination. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[23] M G Saslow,et al. Latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[24] Denise Y P Henriques,et al. Reference frame conversions for repeated arm movements. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[25] R. Johansson,et al. Eye–Hand Coordination in Object Manipulation , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[26] R. Shadmehr,et al. Why Does the Brain Predict Sensory Consequences of Oculomotor Commands? Optimal Integration of the Predicted and the Actual Sensory Feedback , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[27] D. Meyer,et al. Eye-hand coordination: oculomotor control in rapid aimed limb movements. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[28] Jos J. Adam,et al. Reaction time latencies of eye and hand movements in single- and dual-task conditions , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.