Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. Milner,et al. Grasping the past delay can improve visuomotor performance , 2001, Current Biology.
[2] R. Miall,et al. The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[3] W. T. Thach,et al. Throwing while looking through prisms. I. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[4] C. Hall-Haro,et al. Learning motor synergies makes use of information on muscular load. , 2000, Learning & memory.
[5] H. Pick,et al. Visual capture produced by prism spectacles , 1965 .
[6] R B Welch,et al. Evidence for a three-component model of prism adaptation. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.
[7] R. Andersen,et al. Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.
[8] Otmar Bock,et al. Adaptation of aimed arm movements to sensorimotor discordance: evidence for direction-independent gain control , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.
[9] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[10] B. Bridgeman,et al. Alternating prism exposure causes dual adaptation and generalization to a novel displacement , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[11] M. Radeau. Auditory-visual spatial interaction and modularity , 1994, Current psychology of cognition = Cahiers de psychologie cognitive : CPC.
[12] Giuseppe Vallar,et al. The Cognitive and Neural Bases of Spatial Neglect , 2002 .
[13] M. Perenin,et al. Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect , 1998, Nature.
[14] H. H. Mikaelian. Adaptation to rearranged eye-foot coordination , 1970 .
[15] G. M. Redding,et al. Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] Benjamin Wallace,et al. Attention and prism adaptation , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.
[17] B Wallace,et al. Components of prism adaptation in terminal and concurrent exposure: Organization of the eye-hand coordination loop , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[18] J. Tastevin,et al. En partant de l'expérience d'Aristote les déplacements artificiels des parties du corps ne sont pas suivis par le sentiment de ces parties ni par les sensations qu'on peut y produire. , 1937 .
[19] Csaba Veres,et al. Towards a general law of numerical/object identity. Commentary. Author's reply , 2001 .
[20] J. Stein. The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex. , 1992, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[21] Emmanuel Guigon,et al. A Neural Model of Perceptual-Motor Alignment , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[22] E. Heinemann,et al. Über Aufbau und Wandlungen der Wahrnehmungswelt , 1953 .
[23] Gordon M. Redding,et al. Adaptive Spatial Alignment , 1997 .
[24] W. T. Thach,et al. Dynamic coordination of body parts during prism adaptation. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.
[25] Y. Rossetti,et al. Improvement of mental imagery after prism exposure in neglect: a case study. , 1998, Behavioural neurology.
[26] D. H. Warren,et al. Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. , 1980, Psychological bulletin.
[27] Harutune H. Mikaeliaan. Lack of bilateral generalization of adaptation to auditory rearrangement , 1972 .
[28] G. M. Redding,et al. Sources of “overadditivity” in prism adaptation , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.
[29] B Wallace,et al. Adaptive coordination and alignment of eye and hand. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.
[30] D A Wilkinson. Visual-motor control loop: a linear system? , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.
[31] D. Boisson,et al. Does Action Make the Link Between Number and Space Representation? , 2004, Psychological science.
[32] Jordan Grafman,et al. Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .
[33] I P Howard,et al. The Relative Lability of Mobile and Stationary Components in a Visual-Motor Adaptation Task , 1974, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[34] Hermann von Helmholtz,et al. Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1962 .
[35] D. Pélisson,et al. Modifications in end positions of arm movements following short term saccadic adaptation , 1995, Neuroreport.
[36] H. P. Bowditch,et al. A Comparison of Sight and Touch , 1882, The Journal of physiology.
[37] G. M. Redding,et al. Head posture effects in prism adaptation during hallway exposure , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[38] J. Mattingley,et al. The cognitive and neural bases of spatial neglect , 2004 .
[39] B Wallace,et al. Effects of movement duration and visual feedback on visual and proprioceptive components of prism adaptation. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.
[40] S. Ebenholtz. Additivity of aftereffects of maintained head and eye rotations: An alternative to recalibration , 1976 .
[41] Analyse de la réorganisation des coordinations visuomotrices chez l'homme. , 1972 .
[42] Hiroyuki Egusa,et al. Body image as a visuomotor transformation device revealed in adaptation to reversed vision , 2000, Nature.
[43] B Wallace,et al. Calibration and Alignment are Separable: Evidence From Prism Adaptation , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.
[44] G M Redding,et al. Additivity in adaptation to optical tilt. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[45] M Jeannerod,et al. Visuomotor coordination as a dissociable visual function: experimental and clinical evidence. , 1993, Bailliere's clinical neurology.
[46] Y. Rossetti,et al. Prism adaptation to rightward optical deviation improves postural imbalance in left-hemiparetic patients , 2001, Current Biology.
[47] Daniel M Wolpert,et al. Adaptation to a visuomotor shift depends on the starting posture. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.
[48] L. K. Canon,et al. Influence of concurrent and terminal exposure conditions on the nature of perceptual adaptation. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.
[49] G. M. Redding,et al. Strategie Calibration and Spatial Alignment: A Model From Prism Adaptation , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.
[50] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Perceptual distortion contributes to the curvature of human reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.
[51] Alessandro Farnè,et al. In search of biased egocentric reference frames in neglect , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[52] B Craske,et al. No retinal component in prism adaptation. , 1974, Acta psychologica.
[53] L. Jakobson,et al. Trajectories of reaches to prismatically-displaced targets: evidence for “automatic” visuomotor recalibration , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[54] The dissection of intersensory bias: weighting for Radeau , 1994, Current psychology of cognition = Cahiers de psychologie cognitive : CPC.
[55] James G. Taylor. The behavioral basis of perception , 1962 .
[56] C. Bard,et al. Contribution of proprioception for calibrating and updating the motor space. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.
[57] Felice L. Bedford,et al. Constraints on learning new mappings between perceptual dimensions , 1989 .
[58] Yves Rossetti,et al. Dissociated long lasting improvements of straight-ahead pointing and line bisection tasks in two hemineglect patients , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[59] Geoffrey P. Bingham,et al. The rate of adaptation to displacement prisms remains constant despite acquisition of rapid calibration , 1999 .
[60] Joel Goldberg,et al. Immediate correction and adaptation based on viewing a prismatically displaced scene , 1966 .
[61] J J Bloomberg,et al. Variable practice with lenses improves visuo-motor plasticity. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[62] K R Paap,et al. Perceptual consequences of potentiation in the extraocular muscles: an alternative explanation for adaptation to wedge prisms. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[63] Y. Rossetti,et al. Reducing spatial neglect by visual and other sensory manipulations: non-cognitive (physiological) routes to the rehabilitation of a cognitive disorder , 2002 .
[64] Harutune H Mikaellan,et al. Adaptation to Displaced Hearing: A Non-Proprioceptive Change , 1974 .
[65] Benjamin Wallace,et al. First-Trial "Adaptation" to Prism Exposure: Artifact of Visual Capture , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.
[66] Yves Rossetti,et al. Prismatic displacement of vision induces transient changes in the timing of eye-hand coordination , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[67] B Wallace,et al. Adaptive mechanisms in perceptual-motor coordination: components of prism adaptation. , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.
[68] Components of displacement adaptation in acquisition and decay as a function of hand and hall exposure , 1976 .
[69] G. Vallar. Spatial hemineglect in humans , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[70] Benjamin Wallace,et al. Dual Prism Adaptation: Calibration or Alignment? , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.
[71] M. A. Goodale,et al. Eye movements tell only half the story , 2003, Neurology.
[72] G. M. Redding,et al. Effects of Pointing Rate and Availability of Visual Feedback on Visual and Proprioceptive Components of Prism Adaptation. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.
[73] Benjamin Wallace,et al. First-Trial Adaptation to Prism Exposure , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.
[74] R. Held,et al. Adaptation of Disarranged Hand-Eye Coordination Contingent upon Re-Afferent Stimulation , 1958 .
[75] G. M. Redding,et al. Perceptual-motor coordination and prism adaptation during locomotion: A control for head posture contributions , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.
[76] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Are arm trajectories planned in kinematic or dynamic coordinates? An adaptation study , 1995, Experimental Brain Research.
[77] Robert E. Dewar. Adaptation to displaced vision: Variations on the “prismatic-shaping” technique , 1971 .
[78] R B Welch,et al. An examination of the relationship between visual capture and prism adaptation , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.
[79] B. Wallace,et al. Perceptual Adaptation with Selective Reductions of Felt Sensation , 1975 .
[80] B Wallace,et al. Phenomenal versus process explanations of prism aftereffects. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.
[81] George M. Stratton,et al. Upright Vision and the Retinal Image. , 1897 .
[82] R. Lynn,et al. A secular decline in the strength of Spearman’s g in Japan , 1994 .
[83] C. S. Harris,et al. Beware of the Straight-Ahead Shift—A Nonperceptual Change in Experiments on Adaptation to Displaced Vision , 1974, Perception.
[84] A. Maravita,et al. Prism adaptation can improve contralesional tactile perception in neglect , 2003, Neurology.
[85] M. Jeannerod,et al. Adaptation of the Two Arms to opposite Prism Displacements , 1975 .
[86] B Wallace,et al. Effects on prism adaptation of duration and timing of visual feedback during pointing. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.
[87] Kenneth R. Boff,et al. Sensory processes and perception , 1986 .
[88] Oculomotor adaptation to prisms is not simply a muscle potentiation effect , 1975 .
[89] G. M. Redding,et al. Cognitive interference in prism adaptation , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.
[90] K. Heilman,et al. Neglect and Related Disorders , 1984, Seminars in neurology.
[91] P. Sanberg,et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.
[92] Jason B Mattingley,et al. Effects of prismatic adaptation on judgements of spatial extent in peripersonal and extrapersonal space , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[93] J. Baizer,et al. Cerebellar lesions and prism adaptation in macaque monkeys. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.
[94] M. Hallett,et al. Adaptation to lateral displacement of vision in patients with lesions of the central nervous system , 1983, Neurology.
[95] S M Ebenholtz,et al. The Possible Role of Eye-Muscle Potentiation in Several Forms of Prism Adaptation , 1974, Perception.
[96] M. Jeannerod,et al. In search for the egocentric reference. A neurophysiological hypothesis , 1984, Neuropsychologia.
[97] Y. Rossetti,et al. Preserved prism adaptation in bilateral optic ataxia: strategic versus adaptive reaction to prisms , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[98] Benjamin Wallace,et al. An analysis of aftereffects in the measurement of the correction effect , 1973 .
[99] W. T. Thach,et al. Prism adaptation of reaching is dependent on the type of visual feedback of hand and target position , 2001, Brain Research.
[100] M. Goldberg,et al. Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.
[101] Tracy L. Faber,et al. Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching , 1996, Nature.
[102] Alain Vighetto,et al. Optic Ataxia: A Specific Disorder in Visuomotor Coordination , 1983 .
[103] G. Stratton. Vision without inversion of the retinal image. , 1897 .
[104] Y. Rossetti,et al. Ameliorating neglect with prism adaptation: visuo-manual and visuo-verbal measures , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[105] B. Craske,et al. Spatial discordance is a sufficient condition for oculomotor adaptation to prisms: Eye muscle potentiation need not be a factor , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.
[106] B Wallace,et al. Prism Exposure Aftereffects and Direct Effects for Different Movement and Feedback Times , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.
[107] M. Desmurget,et al. An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[108] G. M. Redding,et al. Cognitive Load and Prism Adaptation. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.
[109] S M Ebenholtz,et al. Distance adaptation depends upon plasticity in the oculomotor control system , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.
[110] B Wallace,et al. Prism Adaptation During Target Pointing From Visible and Nonvisible Starting Locations. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.
[111] R. Welch. Perceptual modification : adapting to altered sensory environments / Robert B. Welch , 1978 .
[112] S. Kitazawa,et al. Prism Adaptation of Reaching Movements: Specificity for the Velocity of Reaching , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[113] Dottie M. Clower,et al. Selective use of perceptual recalibration versus visuomotor skill acquisition. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[114] F. Meneghello,et al. Long-lasting amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[115] V. Mountcastle,et al. Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.
[116] Michel Desmurget,et al. The effect of viewing the static hand prior to movement onset on pointing kinematics and variability , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.
[117] H. Pick,et al. Visual and proprioceptive adaptation to optical displacement of the visual stimulus. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.
[118] Felice L. Bedford,et al. Perceptual and cognitive spatial learning. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[119] Yves Rossetti,et al. Bottom-up transfer of sensory-motor plasticity to recovery of spatial cognition: visuomotor adaptation and spatial neglect. , 2003, Progress in brain research.
[120] J. Uhlarik,et al. Role of cognitive factors on adaptation to prismatic displacement. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.
[121] W. T. Thach,et al. Throwing while looking through prisms , 2005 .
[122] B Wallace,et al. Additivity in prism adaptation as manifested in intermanual and interocular transfer , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.
[123] W. T. Thach,et al. Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[124] Jason B. Mattingley,et al. Prismatic adaptation reduces biased temporal order judgements in spatial neglect , 2004, Neuroreport.
[125] Ian P. Howard,et al. Additivity of components of prismatic adaptation , 1974 .
[126] S. Kitazawa,et al. Effects of delayed visual information on the rate and amount of prism adaptation in the human , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.