Learning and memory for personality prototypes.

Although personality traits are commonly assumed to be represented in memory as schemata, little research has addressed whether such schemata can be learned from observation. Subjects in three studies classified 60 person instances into group members and nonmembers as defined by the instances' match to a complex personality prototype. To simulate learning of fuzzy categories, each person instance provided conflicting cues to group membership. Learning for instances' group membership was excellent across studies. In Study 1, frequency of cues indicating group membership was greatly overestimated among nongroup instances. In Study 2, schema-consistent memory bias was revealed for person instances. In Study 3, schemata of consistently positive (or negative) traits were learned faster than arbitrary schemata. The findings implicated frequency sensitivity of memory (Estes, 1986), and a model of probabilistic cued-memory retrieval was developed to account for the effects. The findings were then discussed in relation to everyday cognitive performance.

[1]  L. Horowitz,et al.  The prototype as a construct in abnormal psychology: II. Clarifying disagreement in psychiatric judgments. , 1981 .

[2]  N. Kuiper,et al.  Depressed and nondepressed content self-reference in mild depressives. , 1982, Journal of personality.

[3]  K. R. Hammond,et al.  ANALYZING THE COMPONENTS OF CLINICAL INFERENCE. , 1964, Psychological review.

[4]  L. Horowitz,et al.  The prototype as a construct in abnormal psychology. I. A method for deriving prototypes. , 1981, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[5]  John R. Anderson Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications , 1980 .

[6]  D. J. Schneider,et al.  Prototype salience and implicit personality theories , 1983 .

[7]  W. Mischel,et al.  Traits as prototypes: Effects on recognition memory. , 1977 .

[8]  M. Posner,et al.  On the genesis of abstract ideas. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  R. D'Andrade Memory and the Assessment of Behavior , 1974 .

[10]  H. Lueck,et al.  Influence of Wearing Glasses on Personality Ratings: Crosscultural Validation of an Old Experiment , 1968, Perceptual and motor skills.

[11]  R. L. Solso,et al.  Prototype formation of faces: A case of pseudo-memory , 1981 .

[12]  J. Crocker,et al.  Cognitive Processes in the Revision of Stereotypic Beliefs , 1983 .

[13]  G. A. Miller,et al.  Book Review Nisbett, R. , & Ross, L.Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment.Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1980. , 1982 .

[14]  R. D'Andrade,et al.  Accurate Reflection or Systematic Distortion? A Reply to Block, Weiss, and Thome , 2005 .

[15]  J. Bransford,et al.  Abstraction of visual patterns. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[16]  S. F. Miyamoto,et al.  A Test of Interactionist Hypotheses of Self-Conception , 1956, American Journal of Sociology.

[17]  T. Widiger,et al.  Prototypic typology and the borderline personality disorder. , 1983, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[18]  L. J. Chapman,et al.  Illusory correlation as an obstacle to the use of valid psychodiagnostic signs. , 1969, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[19]  R. N. Tsujimoto Memory bias toward normative and novel trait prototypes. , 1978 .

[20]  C. Osgood,et al.  Factor analysis of meaning. , 1955, Journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Categories and concepts , 1984 .

[22]  F. Hayes-Roth,et al.  Concept learning and the recognition and classification of exemplars , 1977 .

[23]  John D. Bransford,et al.  The abstraction of linguistic ideas , 1971 .

[24]  W. Estes Memory storage and retrieval processes in category learning. , 1986 .

[25]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of Pro-cessing: A Framework for Memory Research , 1975 .

[26]  S. Fiske,et al.  The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .

[27]  J. S. Wiggins,et al.  Personality and Prediction: Principles of Personality Assessment , 1973 .

[28]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Individual Choice Behavior: A Theoretical Analysis , 1979 .

[29]  J. Jenkins,et al.  Abstraction over time of prototypes from distortions of random dot patterns: A replication , 1970 .

[30]  D. Medin,et al.  Linear separability in classification learning. , 1981 .

[31]  S. Rosenzweig III. Need-persistive and ego-defensive reactions to frustration as demonstrated by an experiment on repression. , 1941 .

[32]  G. Āllport Personality: A Psychological Interpretation , 1938 .

[33]  J. Bruner,et al.  THE PERCEPTION OF PEOPLE , 1954 .

[34]  Richard A. Shweder,et al.  How relevant is an individual difference theory of personality? , 1975, Journal of personality.

[35]  Mark W. Altom,et al.  Correlated symptoms and simulated medical classification. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  Kenneth H. Craik,et al.  The Act Frequency Approach to Personality , 1983 .

[37]  H J Eysenck,et al.  Crime and Personality , 1979, Medico-Legal Journal.

[38]  Stephen K. Reed,et al.  Pattern recognition and categorization , 1972 .

[39]  S. Fiske,et al.  Structure and development of social schemata: Evidence from positive and negative transfer effects. , 1985 .

[40]  M. Posner,et al.  Retention of Abstract Ideas. , 1970 .

[41]  P. Hoffman The paramorphic representation of clinical judgment. , 1960, Psychological bulletin.

[42]  E. Brunswik Perception and the Representative Design of Psychological Experiments , 1957 .

[43]  E. E. Jones,et al.  The perception of variability within in-groups and out-groups: Implications for the law of small numbers. , 1980 .

[44]  Sandra Lipsitz Bern Gender Schema Theory: A Cognitive Account of Sex Typing , 1981 .

[45]  E. Brunswik Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology. , 1955, Psychological review.

[46]  Solomon E. Asch,et al.  THINKING ABOUT PERSONS , 1984 .

[47]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .

[48]  G. Bower,et al.  Storage and later recognition of exemplars of concepts , 1973 .

[49]  Crime and Personality. , 1978 .