The perceptual homunculus: the perception of the relative proportions of the human body.

Given that observing one's body is ubiquitous in experience, it is natural to assume that people accurately perceive the relative sizes of their body parts. This assumption is mistaken. In a series of studies, we show that there are dramatic systematic distortions in the perception of bodily proportions, as assessed by visual estimation tasks, where participants were asked to compare the lengths of two body parts. These distortions are not evident when participants estimate the extent of a body part relative to a noncorporeal object or when asked to estimate noncorporal objects that are the same length as their body parts. Our results reveal a radical asymmetry in the perception of corporeal and noncorporeal relative size estimates. Our findings also suggest that people visually perceive the relative size of their body parts as a function of each part's relative tactile sensitivity and physical size.

[1]  P. Enticott,et al.  Own-body perception in body dysmorphic disorder , 2013, Cognitive neuropsychiatry.

[2]  P. Haggard,et al.  Body image distortions in healthy adults. , 2013, Acta psychologica.

[3]  P. Haggard,et al.  Implicit body representations and the conscious body image. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[4]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  A 2.5-D representation of the human hand. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Thérèse Collins,et al.  Visual information and rubber hand embodiment differentially affect reach-to-grasp actions. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[6]  P. Haggard,et al.  Weber's illusion and body shape: anisotropy of tactile size perception on the hand. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  An implicit body representation underlying human position sense , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  P. Haggard,et al.  Rubber Hand Illusions and Size–Weight Illusions: Self-Representation Modulates Representation of External Objects , 2009, Perception.

[9]  T. Jaeger,et al.  Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.

[10]  R. Harald Baayen,et al.  Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R, 1st Edition , 2008 .

[11]  P. Haggard,et al.  Can vision of the body ameliorate impaired somatosensory function? , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  H. Chris Dijkerman,et al.  Somatosensory processing subserving perception and action: Dissociations, interactions, and integration , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[13]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Dynamic modulation of the primary somatosensory cortex during seeing and feeling a touched hand , 2006, NeuroImage.

[14]  G. Moseley,et al.  Distorted body image in complex regional pain syndrome , 2005, Neurology.

[15]  P. Haggard,et al.  Keeping the world a constant size: object constancy in human touch , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  C. Spence,et al.  Early Vision Impairs Tactile Perception in the Blind , 2004, Current Biology.

[17]  P. Haggard,et al.  Visual enhancement of touch in spatial body representation , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Herta Flor,et al.  Phantom-limb pain: characteristics, causes, and treatment , 2002, The Lancet Neurology.

[19]  P. Haggard,et al.  Noninformative vision improves the spatial resolution of touch in humans , 2001, Current Biology.

[20]  S. Gandevia,et al.  Perceptual distortions of the human body image produced by local anaesthesia, pain and cutaneous stimulation , 1999, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  Anne C. Sittig,et al.  The precision of proprioceptive position sense , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  K. Gournay,et al.  Body Dysmorphic Disorder , 1996, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[23]  J. Rosen,et al.  Cognitive-behavioral body image therapy for body dysmorphic disorder. , 1995, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[24]  Henry Dreyfuss,et al.  Measure of Man and Woman: Human Factors in Design , 1993 .

[25]  E. Hollander,et al.  Body dysmorphic disorder. Diagnostic issues and related disorders. , 1992, Psychosomatics.

[26]  J. Kaas,et al.  Consistent features of the representation of the hand in area 3b of macaque monkeys. , 1987, Somatosensory research.

[27]  S. Kahill,et al.  Human figure drawing in adults: An update of the empirical evidence, 1967–1982. , 1984 .

[28]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Does position sense at the elbow reflect a sense of elbow joint angle or one of limb orientation? , 1982, Brain Research.

[29]  B. Green The perception of distance and location for dual tactile pressures , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  J. Kaas,et al.  Multiple representations of the body within the primary somatosensory cortex of primates. , 1979, Science.

[31]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[32]  H. Akaike,et al.  Information Theory and an Extension of the Maximum Likelihood Principle , 1973 .

[33]  S Weinstein,et al.  Intensive and extensive aspects of tactile sensitivity as a function of body part, sex, and laterality , 1968 .

[34]  S. Anstis Apparent Size of Holes felt with the Tongue , 1964, Nature.

[35]  Mabel Ensworth Goudge,et al.  A qualitative and quantitative study of Weber's illusion , 2022 .