The Attentional Drift-Diffusion Model Extends to Simple Purchasing Decisions
暂无分享,去创建一个
Colin Camerer | Dingchao Lu | Ian Krajbich | Antonio Rangel | Colin Camerer | A. Rangel | I. Krajbich | D. Lu
[1] Joseph G. Johnson,et al. A dynamic, stochastic, computational model of preference reversal phenomena. , 2005, Psychological review.
[2] Colin Camerer,et al. Regulation for Conservatives: Behavioral Economics and the Case for 'Asymmetric Paternalism' , 2003 .
[3] R. Ratcliff. A diffusion model account of response time and accuracy in a brightness discrimination task: Fitting real data and failing to fit fake but plausible data , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[4] James L. McClelland,et al. The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.
[5] A. Rangel,et al. Biasing simple choices by manipulating relative visual attention , 2008, Judgment and Decision Making.
[6] R. Bogacz. Optimal decision-making theories: linking neurobiology with behaviour , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[7] Philip L. Smith,et al. Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.
[8] A. Rangel,et al. Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[9] Andreas Glöckner,et al. Network approaches for expert decisions in sports. , 2012, Human movement science.
[10] Jerome R. Busemeyer,et al. Psychological models of deferred decision making , 1988 .
[11] A. Baddeley. Working memory: looking back and looking forward , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[12] Antonio Rangel,et al. Neural computations associated with goal-directed choice , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[13] Andreas Glöckner,et al. How Distinct are Intuition and Deliberation? An Eye-Tracking Analysis of Instruction-Induced Decision Modes , 2009, Judgment and Decision Making.
[14] Philip L. Smith,et al. A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.
[15] Colin F. Camerer,et al. Thinking about Attention in Games: Backward and Forward Induction , 2004 .
[16] Eric J. Johnson,et al. Detecting Failures of Backward Induction: Monitoring Information Search in Sequential Bargaining , 2002, J. Econ. Theory.
[17] N. Chater,et al. Preference reversal in multiattribute choice. , 2010, Psychological review.
[18] Margaret G. Meloy,et al. Choosing an Inferior Alternative , 2006, Psychological science.
[19] Jerome R. Busemeyer,et al. Computational Models of Decision Making , 2003 .
[20] C. Koch,et al. Relative visual saliency differences induce sizable bias in consumer choice , 2012 .
[21] R Ratcliff,et al. A counter model for implicit priming in perceptual word identification. , 1997, Psychological review.
[22] Eric J. Johnson,et al. Information displays and preference reversals , 1988 .
[23] A. Diederich. MDFT account of decision making under time pressure , 2003, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[24] J. Gold,et al. The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.
[25] S. Shimojo,et al. Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[26] James L. McClelland,et al. Loss aversion and inhibition in dynamical models of multialternative choice. , 2004, Psychological review.
[27] R. Ratcliff,et al. Modeling reaction time and accuracy of multiple-alternative decisions , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[28] Roger Ratcliff,et al. A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .
[29] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. A diffusion model account of masking in two-choice letter identification. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] P. Glimcher,et al. The Neurobiology of Decision: Consensus and Controversy , 2009, Neuron.
[31] Adele Diederich,et al. Survey of decision field theory , 2002, Math. Soc. Sci..
[32] G. G. Stokes. "J." , 1890, The New Yale Book of Quotations.
[33] Andreas Glöckner,et al. An eye‐tracking study on information processing in risky decisions: Evidence for compensatory strategies based on automatic processes , 2011 .
[34] J. Townsend,et al. Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. , 1993, Psychological review.
[35] Joseph G. Johnson,et al. Expertise-based differences in search and option-generation strategies. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[36] Marius Usher,et al. Testing Multi-Alternative Decision Models with Non-Stationary Evidence , 2011, Front. Neurosci..
[37] M. Degroot,et al. Measuring utility by a single-response sequential method. , 1964, Behavioral science.
[38] Mark T. Waters,et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits distribution,andreproductioninanymedium,providedtheoriginalauthorandsourcearecredited.Thislicensedoesnot permit commercial exploitation or the creation of derivative works without sp , 2009 .
[39] Colin Camerer,et al. A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[40] Colin Camerer,et al. Transformation of stimulus value signals into motor commands during simple choice , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[41] James L. McClelland,et al. The neurodynamics of choice, value-based decisions and preference reversal , 2008 .
[42] Puiu F. Balan,et al. Attention as a decision in information space , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[43] Diederich,et al. Dynamic Stochastic Models for Decision Making under Time Constraints , 1997, Journal of mathematical psychology.
[44] Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al. Review Frontal Cortex and Reward-guided Learning and Decision-making Figure 1. Frontal Brain Regions in the Macaque Involved in Reward-guided Learning and Decision-making Finer Grained Anatomical Divisions with Frontal Cortical Systems for Reward-guided Behavior , 2022 .
[45] R. Ratcliff,et al. Multialternative decision field theory: a dynamic connectionist model of decision making. , 2001, Psychological review.
[46] Margaret G. Meloy,et al. The goal of consistency as a cause of information distortion. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[47] Atanu Sengupta,et al. Framework of Study , 2013 .
[48] Marios G Philiastides,et al. A mechanistic account of value computation in the human brain , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[49] Ian Krajbich,et al. Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.
[50] J. E. Russo,et al. The distortion of information to support an emerging evaluation of risk , 2011 .
[51] Eric J. Johnson,et al. Aspects of Endowment: A Query Theory of Value Construction , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[52] Christof Koch,et al. The Drift Diffusion Model Can Account for the Accuracy and Reaction Time of Value-Based Choices Under High and Low Time Pressure , 2010, Judgment and Decision Making.
[53] F. Christian. How the Brain Integrates Costs and Benefits During Decision Making , 2010 .