The Riddoch syndrome: insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision.

We have studied a patient, G.Y., who was rendered hemianopic following a lesion affecting the primary visual cortex (area VI), sustained 31 years ago, with the hope of characterizing his ability to discriminate visual stimuli presented in his blind field, both psychophysically and in terms of the brain activity revealed by imaging methods. Our results show that (i) there is a correlation between G.Y.'s capacity to discriminate stimuli presented in his blind field and his conscious awareness of the same stimuli and (ii) that G.Y.'s performance on some tasks is characterized by a marked variability, both in terms of his awareness for a given level of discrimination and in his discrimination for a given level of awareness. The observations on G.Y., and a comparison of his capacities with those of normal subjects, leads us to propose a simple model of the relationship between visual discrimination and awareness. This supposes that the two independent capacities are very tightly coupled in normal subjects (gnosopsia) and that the effect of a VI lesion is to uncouple them, but only slightly. This uncoupling leads to two symmetrical departures, on the one hand to gnosanopsia (awareness without discrimination) and on the other to agnosopsia (discrimination without awareness). Our functional MRI studies show that V5 is always active when moving stimuli, whether slow or fast, are presented to his blind field and that the activity in V5 co-varies with less intense activity in other cortical areas. The difference in cerebral activity between gnosopsia and agnosopsia is that, in the latter, the activity in V5 is less intense and lower statistical thresholds are required to demonstrate it. Direct comparison of the brain activity during individual 'aware' and 'unaware' trials, corrected for the confounding effects of motion, has also allowed us, for the first time, to titrate conscious awareness against brain activity and show that there is a straightforward relationship between awareness and activity, both in individual cortical areas, in this case area V5, and in the reticular activating system. The imaging evidence, together with the variability in his levels of awareness and discrimination, manifested in his capacity to discriminate consciously on some occasions and unconsciously on others, leads us to conclude that agnosopsia, gnosopsia and gnosanopsia are all manifestations of a single condition which we call the Riddoch syndrome, in deference to the British neurologist who, in 1917, first characterized the major aspect of this disability. We discuss the significance of these results in relation to historical views about the organization of the visual brain.

[1]  H. Wilbrand Ophthalmiatrische Beiträge zur Diagnostik der Gehirn-Krankheiten , 2022 .

[2]  Joseph Shaw Bolton The Exact Histological Localisation of the Visual Area of the Human Cerebral Cortex , 1900 .

[3]  G. Riddoch DISSOCIATION OF VISUAL PERCEPTIONS DUE TO OCCIPITAL INJURIES, WITH ESPECIAL REFERENCE TO APPRECIATION OF MOVEMENT , 1917 .

[4]  Ward A. Holden Handbuch der Neurologie. , 1936 .

[5]  G. Holmes Ferrier Lecture - The organization of the visual cortex in man , 1945, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B - Biological Sciences.

[6]  a.R.V.,et al.  Some Papers on the Cerebral Cortex , 1962, Neurology.

[7]  S. Zeki Representation of central visual fields in prestriate cortex of monkey. , 1969, Brain research.

[8]  B. Cragg The topography of the afferent projections in the circumstriate visual cortex of the monkey studied by the Nauta method. , 1969, Vision research.

[9]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. , 1974, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  E. Warrington,et al.  "Blindsight": Vision in a field defect. , 1974, Lancet.

[11]  S. Zeki Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  J. Lund,et al.  Interlaminar connections and pyramidal neuron organisation in the visual cortex, area 17, of the Macaque monkey , 1975 .

[13]  W Singer,et al.  Control of thalamic transmission by corticofugal and ascending reticular pathways in the visual system. , 1977, Physiological reviews.

[14]  K H Ruddock,et al.  Human visual responses in the absence of the geniculo-calcarine projection. , 1980, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Varieties of Residual Experience* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[16]  E. G. Keating,et al.  Residual spatial vision in the monkey after removal of striate and preoccipital cortex , 1980, Brain Research.

[17]  L. Benevento,et al.  The organization of connections between the pulvinar and visual area MT in the macaque monkey , 1983, Brain Research.

[18]  T. Albright Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  K H Ruddock,et al.  Residual vision in patients with retrogeniculate lesions of the visual pathways. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  D. Goodin,et al.  Visual evoked potentials in the investigation of “blindsight” , 1988, Neurology.

[21]  C. Gross,et al.  Afferent basis of visual response properties in area MT of the macaque. I. Effects of striate cortex removal , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  S. Zeki,et al.  The Organization of Connections between Areas V5 and V1 in Macaque Monkey Visual Cortex , 1989, The European journal of neuroscience.

[23]  R H Wurtz,et al.  Functional specialization for visual motion processing in primate cerebral cortex. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[24]  S. Zeki,et al.  A century of cerebral achromatopsia. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  The Ferrier Lecture, 1989 - Outlooks for blindsight: explicit methodologies for implicit processes , 1990, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B. Biological Sciences.

[26]  S. Zeki,et al.  Cerebral akinetopsia (visual motion blindness). A review. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[28]  D Mestre,et al.  [Ambulatory autonomy and visual motion perception in a case of almost total cortical blindness]. , 1992, Revue neurologique.

[29]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Residual vision in a scotoma: implications for blindsight. , 1992, Science.

[30]  M. Poncet,et al.  Perception of optical flow in cortical blindness: A case report , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  P A Salin,et al.  Response selectivity of neurons in area MT of the macaque monkey during reversible inactivation of area V1. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  S Zeki,et al.  Conscious visual perception without V1. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  Jean Bullier,et al.  The Role of Area 17 in the Transfer of Information to Extrastriate Visual Cortex , 1994 .

[35]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[36]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited , 1995, NeuroImage.

[37]  Lawrence Weiskrantz,et al.  Blindsight—Not an Island Unto Itself , 1995 .

[38]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Parameters affecting conscious versus unconscious visual discrimination with damage to the visual cortex (V1). , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  Alan Cowey,et al.  Visual perception and phenomenal consciousness , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[40]  C. N. Guy,et al.  The parallel visual motion inputs into areas V1 and V5 of human cerebral cortex. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Detecting Activations in PET and fMRI: Levels of Inference and Power , 1996, NeuroImage.

[42]  A. Cowey Visual awareness: Still at sea with seeing? , 1996, Current Biology.

[43]  Petra Stoerig,et al.  Varieties of vision: from blind responses to conscious recognition , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.

[44]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Conscious and unconscious processing of visual signals: Psychophysical and fMRI studies , 1996 .

[45]  C. N. Guy,et al.  Motion specific responses from a blind hemifield. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Residual Vision in Multiple Retinal Locations within a Scotoma: Implications for Blindsight , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.