Mental rotation for spatial environment recognition.
暂无分享,去创建一个
A Berthoz | A. Berthoz | F. Gaunet | F Gaunet
[1] M. Tarr,et al. Mental rotation and orientation-dependence in shape recognition , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.
[2] L. Yardley. Contribution of somatosensory information to perception of the visual vertical with body tilt and rotating visual field , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[3] R. Shepard,et al. Transformational studies of the internal representation of three-dimensional objects. , 1974 .
[4] Horst Mittelstaedt,et al. The role of the otoliths in the perception of the orientation of self and world to the vertical , 1991 .
[5] G. Humphrey,et al. Recognizing novel views of three-dimensional objects. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.
[6] M. Tarr,et al. To What Extent Do Unique Parts Influence Recognition Across Changes in Viewpoint? , 1995 .
[7] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.
[8] H. A. Witkin,et al. Studies in space orientation; further experiments on perception of the upright with displaced visual fields. , 1948, Journal of experimental psychology.
[9] M Lipshits,et al. Is there an effect of weightlessness on mental rotation of three-dimensional objects? , 1995, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[10] M. Tarr,et al. When does Human Object Recognition use a Viewer-Centered Reference Frame? , 1990 .
[11] N. Wade. Visual orientation during and after lateral head, body, and trunk tilt , 1968 .
[12] R. Shepard,et al. Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.
[13] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[14] A. Friedman,et al. Shape discriminations of three-dimensional objects depend on the number and location of bends , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[15] F. Attneave,et al. Discriminability of stimuli varying in physical and retinal orientation , 1967 .
[16] B. Bergum,et al. Attention and performance IX , 1982 .
[17] W. Chase,et al. Visual information processing. , 1974 .
[18] M Lipshits,et al. Prolonged weightlessness, reference frames and visual symmetry detection. , 1998, Acta astronautica.
[19] Ross He,et al. Orientation to the vertical in free divers. , 1969 .
[20] Norbert Bischof,et al. Optic-Vestibular Orientation to the Vertical , 1974 .
[21] R. Dolan,et al. Active representation of shape and spatial location in man. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.
[22] Day Rh,et al. Mechanisms involved in visual orientation constancy. , 1969 .
[23] A. Sirigu,et al. Pure Topographical Disorientation: A Definition and Anatomical Basis , 1987, Cortex.
[24] R. Shepard,et al. CHRONOMETRIC STUDIES OF THE ROTATION OF MENTAL IMAGES , 1973 .
[25] J. Paillard. Brain and space , 1991 .
[26] J. Paillard. Motor and representational framing of space , 1991 .
[27] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.
[28] I. Rock,et al. The effect of retinal and phenomenal orientation on the perception of form. , 1957, The American journal of psychology.
[29] T. McNamara,et al. Viewpoint Dependence in Scene Recognition , 1997 .
[30] Effect of gravito-inertial cues on the coding of orientation in pre-attentive vision. , 1995, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.
[31] M. Tarr. Rotating objects to recognize them: A case study on the role of viewpoint dependency in the recognition of three-dimensional objects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[32] L. Matin,et al. Change in Visually Perceived Eye Level without Change in Perceived Pitch , 1998, Perception.
[33] M. Sanders. Handbook of Sensory Physiology , 1975 .
[34] Mislocalizations of Visual Elevation and Visual Vertical Induced by Visual Pitch: the Great Circle Model a , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[35] M. Corballis,et al. Detection of symmetry as a function of angular orientation. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] M. Guerraz,et al. Head Orientation Involvement in Assessment of the Subjective Vertical during Whole Body Tilt , 1998, Perceptual and motor skills.
[37] M Lipshits,et al. Independence of bilateral symmetry detection from a gravitational reference frame. , 1995, Spatial vision.
[38] N. Kanwisher,et al. Mental Imagery of Faces and Places Activates Corresponding Stimulus-Specific Brain Regions , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[39] S. Edelman,et al. Orientation dependence in the recognition of familiar and novel views of three-dimensional objects , 1992, Vision Research.
[40] P. Barraud,et al. Respective Contribution of Orientation Contrast and Illusion of Self-Tilt to the Rod-And-Frame Effect , 1995, Perception.
[41] A. Siegel,et al. The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.
[42] D. Metzler,et al. Mental rotation: effects of dimensionality of objects and type of task. , 1988, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.
[43] C. Marendaz,et al. Solving the rod-and-frame test in a tachistoscopic presentation: Effects of stimulus size and perceptual style , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[44] A GRAYBIEL,et al. PERCEPTION OF THE POSTURAL VERTICAL FOLLOWING PROLONGED BODILY TILT IN NORMALS AND SUBJECTS WITH LABYRINTHINE DEFECTS. , 1964, Acta oto-laryngologica.
[45] M. Corballis,et al. What’s up in mental rotation? , 1976 .
[46] M. Corballis,et al. Mental rotation under head tilt: Factors influencing the location of the subjective reference frame , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.