Ready to Teach or Ready to Learn: A Critique of the Natural Pedagogy Theory
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Kristin Andrews. Do Apes Read Minds?: Toward a New Folk Psychology , 2012 .
[2] Mirko Farina. The evolved apprentice. How evolution made humans unique , 2012, Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences.
[3] G. Csibra,et al. Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[4] H. Bekkering. Rational imitation in preverbal infants Babies may opt for a simpler way to turn on a light after watching an adult do it , 2011 .
[5] F. Marlowe,et al. Hunting and Gathering , 2007 .
[6] L. Bloom,et al. What, when, and how about why: a longitudinal study of early expressions of causality. , 1979, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
[7] Gunilla Stenberg. Why do Infants Look at and Use Positive Information from Some Informants Rather Than Others in Ambiguous Situations? , 2012, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[8] P. Harris,et al. Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. , 2005, Child development.
[9] J. Henrich,et al. The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. , 2001, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.
[10] M. Lewis,et al. Indirect effects and infants' reaction to strangers. , 1984 .
[11] A. Thornton,et al. The evolution of teaching , 2008, Animal Behaviour.
[12] A. Meltzoff. Infant Imitation After a 1-Week Delay: Long-Term Memory for Novel Acts and Multiple Stimuli. , 1988, Developmental psychology.
[13] M. Tomasello,et al. Re-enacting intended acts: Comparing 12- and 18-month-olds , 1999 .
[14] H. Wellman,et al. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.
[15] Barry S. Hewlett,et al. Social learning among Congo Basin hunter–gatherers , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[16] C. Heyes,et al. Social learning in animals : the roots of culture , 1996 .
[17] A. Whiten,et al. Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[18] K. MacDonald. Cross-cultural Comparison of Learning in Human Hunting , 2007, Human nature.
[19] G. Csibra,et al. Beyond rational imitation: learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[20] E. Spelke. Initial knowledge: six suggestions , 1994, Cognition.
[21] Elizabeth A. Ware,et al. Children's sensitivity to the knowledge expressed in pedagogical and nonpedagogical contexts. , 2013, Developmental Psychology.
[22] Frank C Keil,et al. The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[23] M. Tomasello,et al. Young children attribute normativity to novel actions without pedagogy or normative language. , 2011, Developmental science.
[24] G. Csibra,et al. Gaze Following in Human Infants Depends on Communicative Signals , 2008, Current Biology.
[25] T. Walden. Infant social referencing , 1991 .
[26] V. Jaswal,et al. Watch and Learn? Infants Privilege Efficiency Over Pedagogy During Imitative Learning. , 2011, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[27] Marion Vorms,et al. A-not-B Errors: Testing the Limits of Natural Pedagogy Theory , 2012 .
[28] T. Walden,et al. The development of social referencing. , 1988, Child development.
[29] G. Csibra,et al. Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy , 2006 .
[30] A. Gopnik,et al. Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. , 1997, Developmental psychology.
[31] Á. Miklósi,et al. Infant perseverative errors are induced by pragmatic misinterpretation , 2008 .
[32] József Topál,et al. Infants' Perseverative Search Errors Are Induced by Pragmatic Misinterpretation , 2008, Science.
[33] Sunny Shin,et al. Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs ? , 2005 .
[34] Learning "About" Versus Learning "From" Other Minds: Natural Pedagogy and Its Implications , 2008 .
[35] Kathleen H Corriveau,et al. Young children's selective trust in informants , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[36] György Gergely,et al. Communicating Shared Knowledge in Infancy , 2013, Psychological science.
[37] Hannes Rakoczy,et al. Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[38] T. Caro,et al. Is There Teaching in Nonhuman Animals? , 1992, The Quarterly Review of Biology.
[39] Kara D. Sage,et al. Disentangling the Social and the Pedagogical in Infants' Learning about Tool‐Use , 2011 .
[40] Ulf Liszkowski,et al. 18-Month-Olds Predict Specific Action Mistakes Through Attribution of False Belief, Not Ignorance, and Intervene Accordingly. , 2012, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[41] F. Marlowe. Hunter‐gatherers and human evolution , 2005 .
[42] R. Baillargeon,et al. Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs? , 2005, Science.
[43] G. Csibra,et al. Natural pedagogy , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[44] Mark H. Johnson,et al. Eye contact detection in humans from birth , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[45] Andrew G. Young,et al. The hidden structure of overimitation , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[46] Jennifer M. D. Yoon,et al. Communication-induced memory biases in preverbal infants , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[47] Michael M Chouinard. Children's questions: a mechanism for cognitive development. , 2007, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
[48] David Hume. A Treatise of Human Nature: Being an Attempt to introduce the experimental Method of Reasoning into Moral Subjects , 1972 .
[49] Gergely Csibra,et al. Recognizing Communicative Intentions in Infancy , 2010 .
[50] György Gergely,et al. Communicative Function Demonstration induces kind-based artifact representation in preverbal infants , 2010, Cognition.
[51] Joseph Henrich,et al. Prestige-biased cultural learning: bystander's differential attention to potential models influences children's learning , 2012 .
[52] G. Csibra,et al. The social construction of the cultural mind: Imitative learning as a mechanism of human pedagogy , 2005 .
[53] James H. Wirth,et al. Eye Gaze as Relational Evaluation: Averted Eye Gaze Leads to Feelings of Ostracism and Relational Devaluation , 2010, Personality & social psychology bulletin.
[54] G. Gergely,et al. On pedagogy. , 2007, Developmental science.
[55] Diane Poulin-Dubois,et al. Infants prefer to imitate a reliable person. , 2011, Infant behavior & development.
[56] A. Meltzoff. Chapter 16 - The Human Infant as Imitative Generalist: A 20-Year Progress Report on Infant Imitation with Implications for Comparative Psychology , 1996 .
[57] M. Tomasello. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition , 2000 .
[58] Moritz M. Daum,et al. Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants. , 2013, Child development.
[59] David M. Sobel,et al. Children's beliefs about the fantasy/reality status of hypothesized machines. , 2011, Developmental science.
[60] M. Klinnert,et al. The regulation of infant behavior by maternal facial expression , 1984 .