Power of instructions for task implementation: superiority of explicitly instructed over inferred rules
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[2] E. Erdfelder,et al. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses , 2009, Behavior research methods.
[3] J. Houwer,et al. Instruction-based response activation depends on task preparation , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[4] Michael W. Cole,et al. Prefrontal Dynamics Underlying Rapid Instructed Task Learning Reverse with Practice , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[5] Michael W. Cole,et al. Rapid instructed task learning: A new window into the human brain’s unique capacity for flexible cognitive control , 2013, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[6] B. Hommel. Event Files: Evidence for Automatic Integration of Stimulus-Response Episodes , 1998 .
[7] Marcel Brass,et al. Toward a unified framework for research on instructions and other messages: An introduction to the special issue on the power of instructions , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[8] L. Schwabe,et al. Tell me what to do: Stress facilitates stimulus-response learning by instruction , 2018, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[9] Maayan Pereg,et al. The role of working memory in rapid instructed task learning and intention-based reflexivity: An individual differences examination , 2016, Neuropsychologia.
[10] Michael W. Cole,et al. The power of instructions: Proactive configuration of stimulus-response translation. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[11] Hannes Ruge,et al. Rapid formation of pragmatic rule representations in the human brain during instruction-based learning. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.
[12] N. Shahar,et al. Formation of abstract task representations: Exploring dosage and mechanisms of working memory training effects , 2018, Cognition.
[13] Michael W. Cole,et al. The task novelty paradox: Flexible control of inflexible neural pathways during rapid instructed task learning , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[14] Felix D. Schönbrodt,et al. Bayes factor design analysis: Planning for compelling evidence , 2016, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[15] N. Meiran. Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .
[16] Nachshon Meiran,et al. The representation of instructions operates like a prepared reflex: flanker compatibility effects found in first trial following S-R instructions. , 2009, Experimental psychology.
[17] Ayala S Allon,et al. prepdat - An R Package for Preparing Experimental Data for Statistical Analysis , 2016 .
[18] N. Meiran,et al. Structure and Implementation of Novel Task Rules: A Cross-Sectional Developmental Study , 2018, Psychological science.
[19] John Duncan,et al. Goal neglect and Spearman's g: competing parts of a complex task. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[20] Jenny A. Thomas. Meaning in Interaction: An Introduction to Pragmatics , 1995 .
[21] Bernhard Hommel,et al. How object-specific are object files? Evidence for integration by location. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[23] Bernhard Hommel,et al. The Neural Underpinnings of Event-file Management: Evidence for Stimulus-induced Activation of and Competition among Stimulus–Response Bindings , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] A. Treisman. The binding problem , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[25] J. de Houwer,et al. Instruction-based task-rule congruency effects. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[26] N. Meiran,et al. Evidence for instructions-based updating of task-set representations: the informed fadeout effect , 2018, Psychological research.
[27] L. Jacoby. On interpreting the effects of repetition: Solving a problem versus remembering a solution , 1978 .
[28] Senne Braem,et al. Following new task instructions: Evidence for a dissociation between knowing and doing , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[29] N. Shahar,et al. Can we learn to learn? The influence of procedural working-memory training on rapid instructed-task-learning , 2018, Psychological research.
[30] H. Jeffreys,et al. Theory of probability , 1896 .
[31] E. Koechlin,et al. The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition , 1999, Nature.
[32] H. Haider,et al. That’s what task sets are for: shielding against irrelevant information , 2008, Psychological research.
[33] N. Meiran,et al. “Optimal suppression” as a solution to the paradoxical cost of multitasking: examination of suppression specificity in task switching , 2017, Psychological Research.
[34] B. Hommel,et al. Visual attention and the temporal dynamics of feature integration , 2004 .
[35] Nachshon Meiran,et al. The representation of instructions in working memory leads to autonomous response activation: Evidence from the first trials in the flanker paradigm , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[36] N. Meiran,et al. Powerful Instructions: Automaticity Without Practice , 2017 .