Vibrotactile Stimuli Applied to Finger Pads as Biases for Perceived Inertial and Viscous Loads

The perception of the mass and viscosity of an object is based on the dynamic forces applied to our hands when we jiggle or lift the object [1], [2], [3]. This force is commonly assumed to be sensed by kinetic receptors [4] in our muscles or tendons. When jiggling objects, we also experience the cutaneous deformation of our finger pads. In this study, we show that the dynamic vibration on the finger pad influences our perception of mass and viscosity. We experimentally confirm that the vibration on the finger pad, that synchronizes with the hand's accelerations or velocities, enhances the perceived changes in the mass or viscosity when the vibrotactile stimuli and the changes in the mass and viscosity are in the same perceptual direction. For example, when the increased mass and an acceleration-synchronized tactile stimulus-which is a positive bias for the mass-are simultaneously presented to the experiment participants, they respond that the perceived increase in the mass is enhanced. In contrast, when the tactile and proprioceptive stimuli are in perceptually opposite directions, the vibrotactile stimuli cancel the perceived changes in the mass and viscosity. In particular, the effect of the velocity-synchronized vibration on perception is stronger than the effect of the actual viscosity.

[1]  Hikaru Inooka,et al.  Characteristics of human fingertips in the shearing direction , 2000, Biological Cybernetics.

[2]  S. Lederman,et al.  The role of haptic versus visual volume cues in the size-weight illusion , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  Kouta Minamizawa,et al.  A Wearable Haptic Display to Present the Gravity Sensation - Preliminary Observations and Device Design , 2007, Second Joint EuroHaptics Conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems (WHC'07).

[4]  H. Ross,et al.  Sensorimotor mechanisms in weight discrimination , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Satoshi Tadokoro,et al.  A tactile synthesis method using multiple frequency vibrations for representing virtual touch , 2005, 2005 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems.

[6]  M. Turvey Dynamic touch. , 1996, The American psychologist.

[7]  L. Jones,et al.  Perception of force and weight: theory and research. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Interpretation of perceived motor commands by reference to afferent signals. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  M. A. Srinivassan The impact of visual information on the haptic perception of stiffness in virtual environments , 1996 .

[10]  S. Lederman,et al.  The material-weight illusion revisited , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  Allison M. Okamura,et al.  Vibration feedback models for virtual environments , 1998, Proceedings. 1998 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation (Cat. No.98CH36146).

[12]  J. E. Creighton,et al.  Lectures On Human And Animal Psychology , 1896 .

[13]  R. Johansson,et al.  First spikes in ensembles of human tactile afferents code complex spatial fingertip events , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Toshiaki Sugimura,et al.  Active click: tactile feedback for touch panels , 2001, CHI Extended Abstracts.

[15]  Laurent Étienne,et al.  Feeling bumps and holes without a haptic interface: the perception of pseudo-haptic textures , 2004, CHI.

[16]  K. Barrett,et al.  Ganong's Review of Medical Physiology , 2010 .

[17]  J R Flanagan,et al.  Coming to grips with weight perception: Effects of grasp configuration on perceived heaviness , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  H E Ross,et al.  Jiggling a lifted weight does aid discrimination. , 1985, The American journal of psychology.

[19]  Z. Halata,et al.  Anatomy of receptors , 2008 .

[20]  R. Johansson,et al.  Encoding of Direction of Fingertip Forces by Human Tactile Afferents , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Estimation of weights and tensions and apparent involvement of a "sense of effort". , 1974, Experimental neurology.

[22]  Katherine J. Kuchenbecker,et al.  Improving telerobotic touch via high-frequency acceleration matching , 2006, Proceedings 2006 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation, 2006. ICRA 2006..

[23]  Vincent Hayward,et al.  A role for haptics in mobile interaction: initial design using a handheld tactile display prototype , 2006, CHI.

[24]  R. Johansson,et al.  Responses in glabrous skin mechanoreceptors during precision grip in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  P R Burgess,et al.  Perceptions of effort and heaviness during fatigue and during the size-weight illusion. , 1997, Somatosensory & motor research.

[26]  Satoshi Tadokoro,et al.  Alternative Display of Friction Represented by Tactile Stimulation without Tangential Force , 2008, EuroHaptics.

[27]  I. W. Hunter,et al.  Effect of fatigue on force sensation , 1983, Experimental Neurology.

[28]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Effects of surface texture on weight perception when lifting objects with a precision grip , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  J. E. Smith,et al.  On the discrimination of minimal differences in weight. III. The role of frequency , 1937 .