Coupling social attention to the self forms a network for personal significance

Prior social psychological studies show that newly assigned personal significance can modulate high-level cognitive processes, e.g., memory and social evaluation, with self- and other-related information processed in dissociated prefrontal structure: ventral vs. dorsal, respectively. Here, we demonstrate the impact of personal significance on perception and show the neural network that supports this effect. We used an associative learning procedure in which we “tag” a neutral shape with a self-relevant label. Participants were instructed to associate three neutral shapes with labels for themselves, their best friend, or an unfamiliar other. Functional magnetic resonance imaging data were acquired while participants judged whether the shape-label pairs were maintained or swapped. Behaviorally, participants rapidly tagged a neutral stimulus with self-relevance, showing a robust advantage for self-tagged stimuli. Self-tagging responses were associated with enhanced activity in brain regions linked to self-representation [the ventral medial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)] and to sensory-driven regions associated with social attention [the left posterior superior temporal sulcus (LpSTS)]. In contrast, associations formed with other people recruited a dorsal frontoparietal control network, with the two networks being inversely correlated. Responses in the vmPFC and LpSTS predicted behavioral self-bias effects. Effective connectivity analyses showed that the vmPFC and the LpSTS were functionally coupled, with the strength of coupling associated with behavioral self-biases. The data show that assignment of personal social significance affects perceptual matching by coupling internal self-representations to brain regions modulating attentional responses to external stimuli.

[1]  H. Tajfel,et al.  Experiments in intergroup discrimination. , 1970, Scientific American.

[2]  H. Tajfel Experiments in intergroup discrimination. , 1970 .

[3]  T. B. Rogers,et al.  Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. , 1977, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  Anne Locksley,et al.  Social categorization and discriminatory behavior: Extinguishing the minimal intergroup discrimination effect. , 1980 .

[5]  Anthony G. Greenwald,et al.  The Self as a Memory System : Powerful , but Ordinary , 2022 .

[6]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Experimental Tests of the Endowment Effect and the Coase Theorem , 1990, Journal of Political Economy.

[7]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Combining Spatial Extent and Peak Intensity to Test for Activations in Functional Imaging , 1997, NeuroImage.

[8]  Blair T. Johnson,et al.  The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Robust representations for faces: evidence from visual search. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  S. Gallagher Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Neurology: Self-recognition and the right hemisphere , 2001, Nature.

[13]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Mike or me? Self-recognition in a split-brain patient , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[16]  G. Ben-Shakhar,et al.  Dissociations of personally significant and task-relevant distractors inside and outside the focus of attention: a combined behavioral and psychophysiological study. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[17]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  F. Happé Theory of Mind and the Self , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[19]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  F. Bermpohl,et al.  Cortical midline structures and the self , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  Dana Samson,et al.  Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  E. Halgren,et al.  Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others , 2006, Neuron.

[24]  Georg Northoff,et al.  Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self , 2006, NeuroImage.

[25]  K. Pammer,et al.  Attentional shifting and the role of the dorsal pathway in visual word recognition , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  Bryan T. Denny,et al.  Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self from close others. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[27]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[28]  P. Mundy,et al.  Attention, Joint Attention, and Social Cognition , 2007, Current directions in psychological science.

[29]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Aaron C. Koralek,et al.  Two Takes on the Social Brain: A Comparison of Theory of Mind Tasks , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Relevance to self: A brief review and framework of neural systems underlying appraisal , 2007, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[32]  Steven M. Platek,et al.  Neural correlates of self-face recognition: An effect-location meta-analysis , 2008, Brain Research.

[33]  Mark W Woolrich,et al.  Associative learning of social value , 2008, Nature.

[34]  N. Sadato,et al.  Processing of Social and Monetary Rewards in the Human Striatum , 2008, Neuron.

[35]  David J. Turk,et al.  Yours or mine? Ownership and memory , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[36]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[37]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Comparing Families of Dynamic Causal Models , 2010, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[38]  Joy J. Geng,et al.  Contextual Knowledge Configures Attentional Control Networks , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  David J. Turk,et al.  Exploring the effects of ownership and choice on self-memory biases , 2011, Memory.

[40]  David Rudrauf,et al.  Medial PFC Damage Abolishes the Self-reference Effect , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Cognitive contributions of the ventral parietal cortex: an integrative theoretical account , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Perceptual effects of social salience: evidence from self-prioritization effects on perceptual matching. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Extended self: medial prefrontal activity during transient association of self and objects. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[44]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Ascribing beliefs to ingroup and outgroup political candidates: neural correlates of perspective-taking, issue importance and days until the election , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  A Meta-analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Self- and Other Judgments Reveals a Spatial Gradient for Mentalizing in Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.