Optokinetic nystagmus is elicited by curvilinear optic flow during high speed curve driving

When analyzing gaze behavior during curve driving, it is commonly accepted that gaze is mostly located in the vicinity of the tangent point, being the point where gaze direction tangents the curve inside edge. This approach neglects the fact that the tangent point is actually motionless only in the limit case when the trajectory precisely follows the curve's geometry. In this study, we measured gaze behavior during curve driving, with the general hypothesis that gaze is not static, when exposed to a global optical flow due to self-motion. In order to study spatio-temporal aspects of gaze during curve driving, we used a driving simulator coupled to a gaze recording system. Ten participants drove seven runs on a track composed of eight curves of various radii (50, 100, 200 and 500m), with each radius appearing in both right and left directions. Results showed that average gaze position was, as previously described, located in the vicinity of the tangent point. However, analysis also revealed the presence of a systematic optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) around the tangent point position. The OKN slow phase direction does not match the local optic flow direction, while slow phase speed is about half of the local speed. Higher directional gains are observed when averaging the entire optical flow projected on the simulation display, whereas the best speed gain is obtained for a 2° optic flow area, centered on the instantaneous gaze location. The present study confirms that the tangent point is a privileged feature in the dynamic visual scene during curve driving, and underlines a contribution of the global optical flow to gaze behavior during active self-motion.

[1]  Han Collewijn,et al.  Control of human optokinetic nystagmus by the central and peripheral retina: Effects of partial visual field masking, scotopic vision and central retinal scotomata , 1986, Brain Research.

[2]  Richard M Wilkie,et al.  Eye-movements aid the control of locomotion. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[3]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Optokinetic reactions in man elicited by localized retinal motion stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[4]  J. Steen,et al.  Unequal amplitude saccades produced by aniseikonic patterns: Effects of viewing distance , 1995, Vision Research.

[5]  Peter H. Schiller,et al.  The optokinetic response under open and closed loop conditions in the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Rob Gray,et al.  A Two-Point Visual Control Model of Steering , 2004, Perception.

[7]  Hamish G MacDougall,et al.  Functional Assessment of Head–Eye Coordination During Vehicle Operation , 2005, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[8]  Jonathan B. Clark,et al.  Head-eye coordination during simulated orbiter landing. , 2008, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[9]  Holly Carver,et al.  University of Iowa Press , 2008 .

[10]  R. Hetherington The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .

[11]  A. W. Blackwell,et al.  Perception of circular heading from optical flow. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[13]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Differential effects of central versus peripheral vision on egocentric and exocentric motion perception , 1973, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A neural model of visually guided steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  A. V. D. Berg,et al.  Perception of heading , 1993, Nature.

[16]  Daniel J. Hannon,et al.  Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow , 1988, Nature.

[17]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Driving is smoother and more stable when using the tangent point. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[18]  D. R MESTRE,et al.  Ocular Responses to Motion Parallax Stimuli: The Role of Perceptual and Attentional Factors , 1997, Vision Research.

[19]  M. Chattington,et al.  Eye–steering coordination in natural driving , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  Équipe PsyCoTec Driving around bends with manipulated eye-steering coordination , 2008 .

[21]  James A. Crowell,et al.  Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements , 1996, Vision Research.

[22]  D J Hannon,et al.  Eye movements and optical flow. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[23]  A. Remole PERCEPTION WITH AN EYE FOR MOTION , 1987 .

[24]  H Godthelp,et al.  Vehicle Control During Curve Driving , 1986, Human factors.

[25]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Ocular responses to radial optic flow and single accelerated targets in humans , 1999, Vision Research.

[26]  H. Mohellebi,et al.  A High Performance/Low-Cost Mini Driving Simulator Alternative for Human Factor Studies , 2003 .

[27]  David N. Lee,et al.  Where we look when we steer , 1994, Nature.

[28]  Simon K. Rushton,et al.  Optic Flow and Beyond , 2004 .

[29]  Daniel Mestre,et al.  The role of edge lines in curve driving , 2009 .

[30]  K. Hoffmann,et al.  Optokinetic eye movements elicited by radial optic flow in the macaque monkey. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  M. Chattington,et al.  Eye movements coordinated with steering benefit performance even when vision is denied , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  Frederick A. Miles,et al.  Short-latency eye movements: evidence for rapid, parallel processing of optic flow , 2004 .

[33]  James A. Crowell,et al.  Estimating heading during eye movements , 1994, Vision Research.

[34]  D A Gordon,et al.  PERCEPTUAL BASIS OF VEHICLE GUIDANCE , 1966 .

[35]  M. Ernst,et al.  The statistical determinants of adaptation rate in human reaching. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[36]  D Regan,et al.  How do we avoid confounding the direction we are looking and the direction we are moving? , 1982, Science.

[37]  J. Wann,et al.  Steering with or without the flow: is the retrieval of heading necessary? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  Richard Wilkie,et al.  Controlling steering and judging heading: retinal flow, visual direction, and extraretinal information. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  F A Miles,et al.  Ocular Compensation for Self‐Motion. Visual Mechanisms , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[40]  Kan Arai,et al.  Tactile force perception depends on the visual speed of the collision object. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[41]  H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al.  The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[42]  Cynthia Owsley,et al.  Vision and driving , 2010, Vision Research.

[43]  F A Miles,et al.  The neural processing of 3‐D visual information: evidence from eye movements , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[44]  Yiannis Aloimonos,et al.  Vision and action , 1995, Image Vis. Comput..