Searching for threat

In a series of experiments, a visual search task was used to test the idea that biologically relevant threatening stimuli might be recognized very quickly or capture visuo-spatial attention. In Experiment 1, there was evidence for both faster detection and faster search rates for threatening animals than for plants. However, examination of the basis of this effect in Experiment 2 showed that it was not due to threat per se, as detection and search rate advantages were found for pleasant rather than threatening animals compared to plants. In Experiment 3, participants searched for the plants and pleasant and threatening animals used in Experiments 1 and 2, among a fixed heterogeneous selection of non-target items. There was no search rate or detection advantage for threatening animals compared to pleasant animals or plants. The same targets and non-targets as those used in Experiment 3 were also used in Experiment 4. In Experiment 4, participants searched for targets that were presented either close to or distant from an initial fixation point. There was no evidence for a “threat” detection advantage either close to or distant from the cross. Finally, an experiment was conducted in which target categories (fruit, flowers, and animals) were not pre-specified prior to each trial block. There were no differences in reaction times to detect pleasant animals, threatening animals, or fruit. We conclude that the visual search paradigm does not readily reveal any biases that might exist for threatening stimuli in the general population.

[1]  R. Dolan,et al.  A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating "unseen" fear. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  Chris R. Brewin,et al.  Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders , 1989 .

[3]  A. Ohman,et al.  The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  A. Ohman,et al.  Fear appears fast: temporal course of startle reflex potentiation in animal fearful subjects. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[5]  S. Yantis 2. Attentional capture in vision , 1996 .

[6]  J. Tipples,et al.  Orienting to exogenous cues and attentional bias to affective pictures reflect separate processes. , 2000, British journal of psychology.

[7]  Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al.  Just Say No: How Are Visual Searches Terminated When There Is No Target Present? , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  G. Logan,et al.  Converging operations in the study of visual selective attention , 1996 .

[9]  C. Darwin The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals , .

[10]  Michael W. Eysenck,et al.  Trait anxiety, anxious mood, and threat detection. , 1995 .

[11]  U. Neisser VISUAL SEARCH. , 1964, Scientific American.

[12]  Carla J. Groom,et al.  Attentional bias for emotional faces in generalized anxiety disorder. , 1999, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[13]  M. Carrasco,et al.  The eccentricity effect: Target eccentricity affects performance on conjunction searches , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[14]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[15]  K. Mogg,et al.  A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[16]  J. Eccles The emotional brain. , 1980, Bulletin et memoires de l'Academie royale de medecine de Belgique.

[17]  P. Lang International Affective Picture System (IAPS) : Technical Manual and Affective Ratings , 1995 .

[18]  D. Lundqvist,et al.  Unconscious emotion : Evolutionary perspectives, psychophysiological data, and neuropsychological mechanisms , 2000 .

[19]  E. Fox,et al.  Facial Expressions of Emotion: Are Angry Faces Detected More Efficiently? , 2000, Cognition & emotion.

[20]  E. Fox,et al.  Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.