Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers

Humans appear to share with animals a nonverbal counting process. In a nonverbal counting condition, subjects pressed a key a numeral-specified number of times, while saying “the” at every press. The mean number of presses increased as a power function of the target number, with a constant coefficient of variation (c.v.), both within and beyond the proposed subitizing range (1–4 or 5), suggesting small numbers are represented on the same continuum as larger numbers and subject to the same noise process (scalar variability). By contrast, when subjects counted their presses out loud as fast as they could, the c.v. decreased as the inverse square root of the target value (binomial variability instead of scalar variability). The unexpected power-law relation between target value and mean number of presses in nonverbal counting suggests a new hypothesis about the development of the function relating number symbols to mental magnitudes.

[1]  T. Simon,et al.  The foundations of numerical thinking in a brain without numbers , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  Fetterman Jg,et al.  Numerosity discrimination: both time and number matter , 1993 .

[3]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[4]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  S. Carey,et al.  Knowledge of Number: Its Evolution and Ontogeny , 1998, Science.

[6]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[7]  S. S. Stevens,et al.  The scaling of subjective roughness and smoothness. , 1962, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  Robert H. Logie,et al.  Cognitive processes in counting. , 1987 .

[9]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Patrice D. Tremoulet,et al.  Indexing and the object concept: developing `what' and `where' systems , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  Colin Allen,et al.  The evolution of mind , 1998 .

[12]  D A Washburn,et al.  Stroop-Like Effects for Monkeys and Humans: Processing Speed or Strength of Association? , 1994, Psychological science.

[13]  John R. Platt,et al.  Localization of position within a homogeneous behavior chain: Effects of error contingencies , 1971 .

[14]  S. S. Stevens The direct estimation of sensory magnitudes-loudness. , 1956, The American journal of psychology.

[15]  R. Gelman,et al.  Draft of Chapter that will appear in In E. Dupoux (Ed.).(2001). Essay in Honor of Jacques Mehler. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Counting in Animals and Humans , 2001 .

[16]  Susan Carey,et al.  Cognitive Foundations of Arithmetic: Evolution and Ontogenisis , 2001 .

[17]  R M Church,et al.  Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  R M Church,et al.  Time left: linear versus logarithmic subjective time. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[19]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[20]  E S Spelke,et al.  Core knowledge. , 2000, The American psychologist.

[21]  J G Fetterman,et al.  Numerosity discrimination: both time and number matter. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[22]  P. Starkey,et al.  The development of subitizing in young children , 1995 .

[23]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Timing and time perception. , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  Thomas K. Landauer,et al.  Determinants of reaction time for digit inequality judgments , 1973 .

[25]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.

[26]  F. Gregory Ashby,et al.  Subitizing: Magical numbers or mere superstition? , 1992, Psychological research.

[27]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[28]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  A Rational-Constructivist Account of Early Learning About Numbers and Objects , 1993 .

[29]  C R Gallistel,et al.  Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale , 2001, Psychological science.

[30]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[31]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The Cultural and Evolutionary History of the Real Numbers , 2001 .

[32]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Single-digit and two-digit Arabic numerals address the same semantic number line , 1999, Cognition.

[33]  D M Rumbaugh,et al.  Summation in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[34]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Measurement Estimation: Learning to Map the Route From Number to Quantity and Back , 1998 .

[35]  H S Terrace,et al.  Representation of the numerosities 1-9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[36]  G. Mandler,et al.  Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[37]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  The Mathematical Brain , 1999 .