Dynamic cortical networks of verbal and spatial working memory: effects of memory load and task practice.

Working memory (WM), the ability to briefly retain and manipulate information in mind, is central to intelligent behavior. Here we take advantage of the high temporal resolution of electrophysiological measures to obtain a millisecond timescale view of the activity induced in distributed cortical networks by tasks that impose significant WM demands. We examined how these networks are affected by the type and amount of information to be remembered, and by the amount of task practice. Evoked potentials (EPs) were obtained from eight subjects performing spatial and verbal versions of a visual n-back WM task (n = 1, 2, 3) on each of three testing days. In well-trained subjects, WM tasks elicited transient responses reflecting different subcomponents of task processing, including transient (lasting 0.02-0.3 s) task-sensitive and load-sensitive EPs, as well as sustained responses (lasting 1-1.5 s), including the prestimulus Contingent Negative Variation (CNV), and post-stimulus frontal and parietal Slow Waves. The transient responses, with the exception of the P300, differed between the verbal and spatial task versions, and between trials with different response requirements. The P300 and the Slow Waves were not affected by task version but were affected by increased WM load. These results suggest that WM emerges from the formation of a dynamic cortical network linking task-specific processes with non-specific, capacity-limited, higher-order attentional processes. Practice effects on the EPs suggested that practice led to the development of a more effective cognitive strategy for dealing with lower-order aspects of task processing, but did not diminish demands made on higher order processes. Thus a simple WM task is shown to be composed of numerous elementary subsecond neural processes whose characteristics vary with type and amount of information being remembered, and amount of practice.

[1]  A S Gevins,et al.  Effects of prolonged mental work on functional brain topography. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[2]  Daphne N. Yu,et al.  High-resolution EEG mapping of cortical activation related to working memory: effects of task difficulty, type of processing, and practice. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. , 1998, Science.

[4]  R. Knight,et al.  Gating of somatosensory input by human prefrontal cortex , 1990, Brain Research.

[5]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. , 1993, Science.

[6]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[7]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Coactivation of prefrontal cortex and inferior parietal cortex in working memory tasks revealed by 2DG functional mapping in the rhesus monkey , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  J. Grafman,et al.  Distinctions and similarities among working memory processes: an event-related potential study. , 1992, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[9]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Verbal Working Memory Load Affects Regional Brain Activation as Measured by PET , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Posner,et al.  Attentional Mechanisms and Conscious Experience , 1992 .

[12]  F. Crick Function of the thalamic reticular complex: the searchlight hypothesis. , 1984, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  T W Picton,et al.  The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[14]  G. Deutsch,et al.  Cerebral blood flow evidence of right frontal activation in attention demanding tasks. , 1987, The International journal of neuroscience.

[15]  M. Just,et al.  The psychology of reading and language comprehension , 1986 .

[16]  R. Desimone,et al.  Parallel neuronal mechanisms for short-term memory. , 1994, Science.

[17]  A. Gevins,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of component processes in human working memory. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[18]  Mark C. Detweiler,et al.  A Connectionist/Control Architecture for Working Memory , 1988 .

[19]  A. A. Wijers,et al.  Memory search and visual spatial attention: an event-related brain potential analysis. , 1985, Acta psychologica.

[20]  P. Roland Cortical regulation of selective attention in man. A regional cerebral blood flow study. , 1982, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Arthur P. Shimamura,et al.  Memory for the temporal order of events in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  J M Fuster,et al.  Neuronal firing in the inferotemporal cortex of the monkey in a visual memory task , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  S E Petersen,et al.  A positron emission tomography study of the short-term maintenance of verbal information , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  S C Rao,et al.  Integration of what and where in the primate prefrontal cortex. , 1997, Science.

[25]  R S Berndt,et al.  Modality-specific processing streams in verbal working memory: evidence from spatio-temporal patterns of brain activity. , 1997, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[26]  F. D. Silva Neural mechanisms underlying brain waves: from neural membranes to networks. , 1991 .

[27]  J. Sergent,et al.  Positron emission tomography study of letter and object processing: empirical findings and methodological considerations. , 1992, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  B. Milner,et al.  Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[30]  D. Ruchkin,et al.  Short-term memory storage and retention: an event-related brain potential study. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[31]  W Lang,et al.  Cortical DC potential shifts accompanying auditory and visual short-term memory. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[32]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human prefrontal cortex activation during a spatial working memory task. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  John Duncan,et al.  A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex , 1993, Nature.

[35]  M. Raichle,et al.  Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography , 1991, Nature.

[36]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  S. Bressler,et al.  Episodic multiregional cortical coherence at multiple frequencies during visual task performance , 1993, Nature.

[38]  S. Bressler,et al.  Shadows of thought: shifting lateralization of human brain electrical patterns during brief visuomotor task. , 1983, Science.

[39]  A. Aertsen,et al.  Dynamics of neuronal interactions in monkey cortex in relation to behavioural events , 1995, Nature.

[40]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[41]  M. Just,et al.  From the SelectedWorks of Marcel Adam Just 1992 A capacity theory of comprehension : Individual differences in working memory , 2017 .

[42]  H. Begleiter,et al.  A neurophysiologic correlate of visual short-term memory in humans. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[43]  B. Milner,et al.  The relationship of working memory to the immediate recall of stories following unilateral temporal or frontal lobectomy , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Edward Awh,et al.  Spatial versus Object Working Memory: PET Investigations , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  P. Roland,et al.  Activation of Multi‐modal Cortical Areas Underlies Short‐term Memory , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[46]  G E Raney,et al.  Monitoring changes in cognitive load during reading: an event-related brain potential and reaction time analysis. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[47]  A. Starr,et al.  Disordered auditory short-term memory in man and event-related potentials. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[49]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET , 1993, Nature.

[50]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal cortex gating of auditory transmission in humans , 1989, Brain Research.

[51]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Neuronal correlate of pictorial short-term memory in the primate temporal cortexYasushi Miyashita , 1988, Nature.

[52]  A. D. Fisk,et al.  The effects of practice and task structure on components of the event-related brain potential. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[53]  A Baddeley,et al.  Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning andmotivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47-89). , 1974 .

[54]  Patrick C. Kyllonen,et al.  Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! , 1990 .

[55]  L. McEvoy,et al.  High resolution evoked potential imaging of the cortical dynamics of human working memory. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[56]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[57]  A. S. Gevins,et al.  Ocular Artifact Minimization by Adaptive Filtering , 1994, IEEE Seventh SP Workshop on Statistical Signal and Array Processing.

[58]  J. Fuster,et al.  Inferotemporal neurons distinguish and retain behaviorally relevant features of visual stimuli. , 1981, Science.

[59]  Michael E. Smith,et al.  Monitoring Working Memory Load during Computer-Based Tasks with EEG Pattern Recognition Methods , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[60]  Frank E. Gomer,et al.  Electrocortical Activity and Operator Workload: A Comparison of Changes in the Electroencephalogram and in Event-Related Potentials , 1981 .

[61]  D B Lindsley,et al.  Brain wave components of the contingent negative variation in humans. , 1976, Science.

[62]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A Parametric Study of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Human Working Memory , 1996, NeuroImage.

[63]  M. Farah,et al.  A unified account of cognitive impairments following frontal lobe damage: the role of working memory in complex, organized behavior. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[64]  S. Sternberg Memory-scanning: mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. , 1969, American scientist.

[65]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Event related potentials and EEG components in a semantic memory search task. , 1992, Psychophysiology.

[66]  A S Gevins,et al.  Human neuroelectric patterns predict performance accuracy. , 1987, Science.

[67]  A. Mecklinger,et al.  Event-related potentials reveal topographical and temporal distinct neuronal activation patterns for spatial and object working memory. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[68]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[69]  J. Tecce Contingent negative variation (CNV) and psychological processes in man. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.

[70]  J. Jonides,et al.  Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[71]  D. Manoach,et al.  Prefrontal cortex fMRI signal changes are correlated with working memory load , 1997, Neuroreport.

[72]  C D Wickens,et al.  The Event-Related Brain Potential as an Index of Display-Monitoring Workload , 1980, Human factors.

[73]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[74]  D. Strayer,et al.  Attentional Requirements of Automatic and Controlled Processing , 1990 .

[75]  A S Gevins,et al.  Neurocognitive pattern analysis of a visuospatial task: rapidly-shifting foci of evoked correlations between electrodes. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[76]  V. Menon,et al.  Local estimate of surface Laplacian derivation on a realistically shaped scalp surface and its performance on noisy data. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[77]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory , 1993, Nature.

[78]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Dissociation of Storage and Rehearsal in Verbal Working Memory: Evidence From Positron Emission Tomography , 1996 .

[79]  M. Kutas,et al.  Who Did What and When? Using Word- and Clause-Level ERPs to Monitor Working Memory Usage in Reading , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[80]  D. Ruchkin,et al.  Working memory and preparation elicit different patterns of slow wave event-related brain potentials. , 1995, Psychophysiology.