Reduced top-down connectivity as an underlying mechanism for psychotic experiences in healthy people

Perception results from our brain’s ability to make predictive models of sensory information. Recently, it has been proposed that psychotic traits may be linked to impaired predictive processes. Here, we examine the brain dynamics underlying prediction formation in a population of healthy individuals with a range of psychotic experiences. We designed a novel paradigm, which incorporated both stable and volatile sound sequences by manipulating their probability. We measured prediction error with electroencephalography and gauged prediction formation explicitly by behaviourally recording sensory ‘regularity’ learning errors. Critically, we show that top-down frontotemporal connectivity may be a neural mechanism by which impaired regularity learning influences psychotic experiences. These findings further our understanding of the neurobiological underpinnings of prediction formation and provide evidence for a continuum of psychosis in the healthy, non-clinical population. One Sentence Summary Healthy individuals with psychotic experiences have impaired sensory learning, mediated by reduced top-down frontotemporal connectivity.

[1]  N. Squires,et al.  Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[2]  J B Poline,et al.  Evidence for abnormal cortical functional connectivity during working memory in schizophrenia. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.

[3]  A. Borst Seeing smells: imaging olfactory learning in bees , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  Oliver J. Hulme,et al.  Altered auditory processing and effective connectivity in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome , 2018, Schizophrenia Research.

[5]  M. Chait,et al.  Enhanced deviant responses in patterned relative to random sound sequences , 2017, Cortex.

[6]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling of evoked potentials: A reproducibility study , 2007, NeuroImage.

[7]  Maxine T. Sherman,et al.  Prior expectations facilitate metacognition for perceptual decision , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[8]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The mismatch negativity: A review of underlying mechanisms , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[9]  Michael Wagner,et al.  Prediction of Psychosis by Mismatch Negativity , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[10]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Computational Anatomy of Psychosis , 2013, Front. Psychiatry.

[11]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dysconnection in Schizophrenia: From Abnormal Synaptic Plasticity to Failures of Self-monitoring , 2009, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[12]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The functional anatomy of the MMN: A DCM study of the roving paradigm , 2008, NeuroImage.

[13]  Jérémie Mattout,et al.  Multivariate source prelocalization (MSP): Use of functionally informed basis functions for better conditioning the MEG inverse problem , 2005, NeuroImage.

[14]  C. Mathys,et al.  F150. OVERESTIMATING ENVIRONMENTAL VOLATILITY INCREASES SWITCHING BEHAVIOR AND IS LINKED TO ACTIVATION OF DORSOLATERAL PREFRONTAL CORTEX IN SCHIZOPHRENIA , 2017, Schizophrenia Bulletin.

[15]  A. Üçok,et al.  Mismatch negativity at acute and post-acute phases of first-episode schizophrenia , 2008, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.

[16]  Z. Dienes Bayesian Versus Orthodox Statistics: Which Side Are You On? , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[17]  M S Gazzaniga,et al.  Organization of the human brain. , 1989, Science.

[18]  M. Garrido,et al.  Auditory prediction errors and auditory white matter microstructure associated with psychotic-like experiences in healthy individuals , 2019, Brain Structure and Function.

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modeling for EEG and MEG , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[20]  E. Schröger,et al.  Temporal regularity facilitates higher‐order sensory predictions in fast auditory sequences , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.

[21]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Outlier Responses Reflect Sensitivity to Statistical Structure in the Human Brain , 2013, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[22]  S. Braeutigam,et al.  Abnormal intrinsic and extrinsic connectivity within the magnetic mismatch negativity brain network in schizophrenia: A preliminary study , 2012, Schizophrenia Research.

[23]  E. Schröger,et al.  Differential Contribution of Frontal and Temporal Cortices to Auditory Change Detection: fMRI and ERP Results , 2002, NeuroImage.

[24]  A. Provost,et al.  Lasting first impressions: A conservative bias in automatic filters of the acoustic environment , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[26]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Expected and Unexpected Uncertainty: ACh and NE in the Neocortex , 2002, NIPS.

[27]  Sanghoon Han,et al.  The Inferior Parietal Lobule and Recognition Memory: Expectancy Violation or Successful Retrieval? , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  D. Braff,et al.  The relationship between preattentive sensory processing deficits and age in schizophrenia patients , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[29]  M. Garrido,et al.  Sensory prediction errors in the continuum of psychosis , 2018, Schizophrenia Research.

[30]  Tyrone D. Cannon,et al.  The prodromal questionnaire (PQ): Preliminary validation of a self-report screening measure for prodromal and psychotic syndromes , 2005, Schizophrenia Research.

[31]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A Bayesian Foundation for Individual Learning Under Uncertainty , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[32]  G. Rees,et al.  Adults with autism over-estimate the volatility of the sensory environment , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  C. Summerfield,et al.  A Neural Representation of Prior Information during Perceptual Inference , 2008, Neuron.

[34]  B. Rockstroh,et al.  Topography of the auditory P300 in schizotypal personality , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[35]  S. Grimm,et al.  Functional dissociation between regularity encoding and deviance detection along the auditory hierarchy , 2016, The European journal of neuroscience.

[36]  D. Linden,et al.  Evidence of absence: no relationship between behaviourally measured prediction error response and schizotypy , 2017, Cognitive neuropsychiatry.

[37]  J. Os,et al.  Psychotic symptoms in non-clinical populations and the continuum of psychosis , 2002, Schizophrenia Research.

[38]  J. Changeux,et al.  A Neuronal Model of Predictive Coding Accounting for the Mismatch Negativity , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Nicholas D. Duran,et al.  Prediction during statistical learning, and implications for the implicit/explicit divide , 2012, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[40]  Erich Schröger,et al.  Regularity Extraction and Application in Dynamic Auditory Stimulus Sequences , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Anxious individuals have difficulty learning the causal statistics of aversive environments , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling of evoked responses: The role of intrinsic connections , 2007, NeuroImage.

[43]  J. Sweeney,et al.  Impact of antipsychotic treatment on attention and motor learning systems in first-episode schizophrenia. , 2015, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[44]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Electromagnetic source reconstruction for group studies , 2008, NeuroImage.

[45]  Karl J. Friston Functional integration and inference in the brain , 2002, Progress in Neurobiology.

[46]  O. Bertrand,et al.  Implicit learning of predictable sound sequences modulates human brain responses at different levels of the auditory hierarchy , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[47]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Mismatch negativity (MMN) deficiency: a break-through biomarker in predicting psychosis onset. , 2015, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[48]  J. van os,et al.  Psychosis as a transdiagnostic and extended phenotype in the general population , 2016, World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association.

[49]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial Attention, Precision, and Bayesian Inference: A Study of Saccadic Response Speed , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  Karl J. Friston The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[51]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Impaired prefrontal synaptic gain in people with psychosis and their relatives during the mismatch negativity , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[52]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  The influence of prior expectations on emotional face perception in adolescence. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[53]  H. Yabe,et al.  Deviant Matters: Duration, Frequency, and Intensity Deviants Reveal Different Patterns of Mismatch Negativity Reduction in Early and Late Schizophrenia , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[54]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Brain responses in humans reveal ideal observer-like sensitivity to complex acoustic patterns , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[55]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Exploration, novelty, surprise, and free energy minimization , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[56]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical advantages and practical ramifications , 2017, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[57]  D. Umbricht,et al.  Mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis , 2005, Schizophrenia Research.

[58]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Comparing Families of Dynamic Causal Models , 2010, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[59]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Learning the value of information in an uncertain world , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[61]  Matthew C. Keller,et al.  Increased sensitivity in neuroimaging analyses using robust regression , 2005, NeuroImage.

[62]  Danai Dima,et al.  Impaired top-down processes in schizophrenia: A DCM study of ERPs , 2010, NeuroImage.

[63]  R. Croft,et al.  Schizotypy and auditory mismatch negativity in a non-clinical sample of young adults , 2016, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[64]  L. Elliot Hong,et al.  Refining the Predictive Pursuit Endophenotype in Schizophrenia , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[65]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Multiple sparse priors for the M/EEG inverse problem , 2008, NeuroImage.

[66]  Rick A Adams,et al.  Computational Psychiatry: towards a mathematically informed understanding of mental illness , 2015, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[67]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Repetition suppression and plasticity in the human brain , 2009, NeuroImage.

[68]  Z. Šidák Rectangular Confidence Regions for the Means of Multivariate Normal Distributions , 1967 .

[69]  Zachary P. Kilpatrick,et al.  Evidence accumulation and change rate inference in dynamic environments , 2016, bioRxiv.

[70]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic Causal Modeling of the Response to Frequency Deviants , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[71]  M. Grube,et al.  Weighting of neural prediction error by rhythmic complexity: A predictive coding account using mismatch negativity , 2019, The European journal of neuroscience.

[72]  E. Bramon,et al.  P300 waveform and dopamine transporter availability: a controlled EEG and SPECT study in medication-naive patients with schizophrenia and a meta-analysis , 2013, Psychological Medicine.

[73]  I. Winkler,et al.  What controls gain in gain control? Mismatch negativity (MMN), priors and system biases , 2014, Brain Topography.

[74]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[75]  I. Winkler,et al.  Mismatch negativity (MMN) to pitch change is susceptible to order-dependent bias , 2014, Front. Neurosci..

[76]  Klaas E. Stephan,et al.  Ketamine Affects Prediction Errors about Statistical Regularities: A Computational Single-Trial Analysis of the Mismatch Negativity , 2019, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[77]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Bayesian inferences about the self (and others): A review , 2014, Consciousness and Cognition.

[78]  P. Paavilainen,et al.  Predictive coding of phonological rules in auditory cortex: A mismatch negativity study , 2016, Brain and Language.

[79]  J. Dickey,et al.  The Weighted Likelihood Ratio, Sharp Hypotheses about Chances, the Order of a Markov Chain , 1970 .

[80]  Klaas E. Stephan,et al.  Inferring on the Intentions of Others by Hierarchical Bayesian Learning , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[81]  Albert R. Powers,et al.  Pavlovian conditioning–induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors , 2017, Science.

[82]  P. DeRosse,et al.  Examining the Psychosis Continuum , 2015, Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports.