Functional properties of parietal visual neurons: radial organization of directionalities within the visual field

Parietal visual neurons (PVNs) were studied in waking monkeys as they executed a simple fixation-detection task. Test visual stimuli of varied direction, speed, and extent were presented during the fixation period; these stimuli did not control behavior. Most PVNs subtend large, bilateral receptive fields and are exquisitely sensitive to stimulus motion and direction but insensitive to stimulus speed. The directional preferences of PVNs along meridians are opponently organized, with the preferred directions pointing either inward toward or outward away from the fixation point. Evidence presented in the preceding paper (Motter et al., 1987) indicates that opponent directionality along a single meridian is produced by a feed-forward inhibition of 20 degrees-30 degrees spatial extent. The observations fit a double-Gaussian model of superimposed but unequal excitatory and inhibitory receptive fields: When the former is larger, inward directionality results; when smaller, outward directionality results. We examine here the distribution of the meridional directional preferences in the visual field. Tests showed that opponent organization is not produced by differences in local directional properties in different parts of the receptive field. The distribution of response intensities from one meridian to another is adequately described by a sine wave function. These data indicate a best radial direction for each neuron with a broad distribution of response intensities over successive meridians. Thus, any single PVN, with rare exceptions, cannot signal radial stimulus direction precisely. We then determined how accurately the population response predicted radial stimulus direction by the application of a linear vector summation model. The resulting population vector varied from stimulus direction by an average of 9 degrees. Whether or not the perception of the direction of motion depends upon a population vector remains uncertain. PVNs are especially sensitive to object movement in the visual surround, particularly in the periphery of the visual field. This, combined with their large receptive fields and their wide but flat sensitivity to stimulus speed, makes them especially sensitive to optic flow. This is discussed in relation to the role of the parietal visual system in the visual guidance of projected movements of the arm and hand, in the guidance of locomotion, and in evoking the illusion of vection.

[1]  R. Bálint Seelenlähmung des “Schauens”, optische Ataxie, räumliche Störung der Aufmerksamkeit. pp. 67–81 , 1909 .

[2]  Richard M Michaels,et al.  STATIC AND DYNAMIC VISUAL FIELDS IN VEHICULAR GUIDANCE , 1965 .

[3]  D A Gordon,et al.  Static and dynamic visul fields in human space perception. , 1965, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[4]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[5]  C. I. Howarth,et al.  The Movement of the Hand towards a Target , 1972, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  K. Mardia Statistics of Directional Data , 1972 .

[7]  J. Lishman,et al.  The Autonomy of Visual Kinaesthesis , 1973, Perception.

[8]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Optical Velocity Patterns, Velocity-Sensitive Neurons, and Space Perception: A Hypothesis , 1974, Perception.

[9]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  David N. Lee Visual proprioceptive control of stance , 1975 .

[11]  G. Johansson Studies on Visual Perception of Locomotion , 1977, Perception.

[12]  C Bonnet,et al.  Visual Motion Detection Models: Features and Frequency Filters , 1977, Perception.

[13]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Activity of postcentral cortical neurons of the monkey during conditioned movements of a deafferented limb , 1979, Brain Research.

[14]  J. Paillard,et al.  Contribution of Positional and Movement Cues to Visuomotor Reaching in Split-Brain Monkey , 1979 .

[15]  E. Batschelet Circular statistics in biology , 1981 .

[16]  B. C. Motter,et al.  The influence of attentive fixation upon the excitability of the light- sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex , 1981, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  B. C. Motter,et al.  The functional properties of the light-sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex studied in waking monkeys: foveal sparing and opponent vector organization , 1981, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  J. Paillard,et al.  Visual motion cues in prismatic adaptation: evidence of two separate and additive processes. , 1981, Acta psychologica.

[19]  Claude Bonnet,et al.  Thresholds of Motion Perception , 1982 .

[20]  J. Seal,et al.  Activity of neurons in area 5 during a simple arm movement in monkeys before and after deafferentation of the trained limb , 1982, Brain Research.

[21]  D Regan,et al.  How do we avoid confounding the direction we are looking and the direction we are moving? , 1982, Science.

[22]  John F. Kalaska,et al.  Spatial coding of movement: A hypothesis concerning the coding of movement direction by motor cortical populations , 1983 .

[23]  R. Andersen,et al.  The influence of the angle of gaze upon the excitability of the light- sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex , 1983, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  Alain Vighetto,et al.  Optic Ataxia: A Specific Disorder in Visuomotor Coordination , 1983 .

[25]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Single visual neurons code opposing motion independent of direction. , 1983, Science.

[26]  V. Mountcastle The visual functions of the parietal lobe , 1984, Behavioural Brain Research.

[27]  J E Cutting,et al.  Visual flow and direction of locomotion. , 1985, Science.

[28]  D. Regan,et al.  Visual flow and direction of locomotion. , 1985, Science.

[29]  J. Paillard,et al.  Static versus Kinetic Visual Cues for the Processing of Spatial Relationships , 1985 .

[30]  J. Allman,et al.  Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of local and wide-field movements in the superior temporal visual areas of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  A. Georgopoulos On reaching. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  斎藤 秀昭,et al.  Integration of Direction Signals of Image Motion in the Superior Temporal Sulcus of the Macaque Monkey , 1987 .

[34]  B. C. Motter,et al.  Functional properties of parietal visual neurons: mechanisms of directionality along a single axis , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.